Waveforms and mechanisms in neuromodulation


Introduction

All of neuromodulation is dependent on the delivery of a waveform comprised of electromagnetic fields to nervous system tissues in the human body. We are setting aside for now the types of neuromodulation working within the realm of light, ultrasonics, or any other forms of manipulating the function of a nervous system. Because the nervous system is generally thought to rely on information transfer to effect its processing of the external world and to make predictions for movement and behavior, and because that information appears to be carried in numbers and patterns of discrete ionic flux along axons from one location to another (referred to hereafter as action potentials or APs), it makes sense to try to intervene in that information transfer for therapeutic benefit when needed and where possible. Such interventions will necessarily be made with a certain frequency and pulse length and shape of pulse, as well as amplitude. These parameters are unavoidable. And so it is imperative that we understand how such variations in these basic parameters affect nervous tissue. A tremendous amount of knowledge has been generated along these lines and many textbooks cover much of this material (e.g., Ref. [ ]) and we will not use this space to go into much of that detail in depth other than also to mention that an excellent summary of the physics and electrochemistry of that process is covered in this chapter. Here, we look at the shapes and cadence of the waveforms themselves and where they have been used already commercially, and then examine some aspects of how the waveforms may affect the actual mechanisms of action on the nervous tissue that we largely measure only on a behavioral level.

Waveform shapes

Much like analyzing how we have come to describe music, we can break down waveforms into a few very fundamental aspects. An electrical field-creating source transfers the field to the tissue via a metallic electrode contact, or more than one and is either “on” or “off.” A note in music either occurs or it does not occur in time. By this binary reality, the device either is creating an electric field of some sort or is not, at any given moment. If it had been “on” and is now “off,” there may also be lingering effects from it having been “on” that are still operative on the tissue. Conversely, if the tissues had been affected in the past by a stimulus, they are unlikely to be in exactly the same state when the stimulus is again “on” that they were in at previous times. So many variables, each with their own time constants, are brought into the mix of any type of stimulus on tissue that when one contemplates how any of the effects can be predicted the problem quickly can seem to be overwhelming. Cell membranes, secondary messengers, a variety of transmitters and receptors, all with their own time constants, longer term changes in receptor up- and downregulation, and other proteomic and genomic changes dynamically altered over multiple time scales all make the problem staggering. Moreover, many of these variables remain unknown in their values before, during, and after our attempts are made to analyze the process. As such, we are often left with only the behavioral level to measure, not by choice, but by necessity.

But as in music, notes are there for a time, or not. They linger in the vibrations of air for a time—variable times. They have different frequencies, are held on for varying amounts of time, and are strung together in time using a variety of cadences, rigidly rhythmic or less so. And a note can have a variety of characteristics like attack shape which alter our perception of it. That is pretty much everything needed to describe music in any composition, and likewise the nature of the waveform used in any neuromodulation device.

Currently, neuromodulation devices use, for the most part, square waves that are delivered in a static frequency with a static pulsewidth either continuously without pause, or in packets as so-called “bursts”. Amplitudes are adjusted in programming sessions using feedback from patients most of the time or based on prior anecdotal information regarding what amplitudes seemed to have been beneficial ( Fig. 6.1 ).

Figure 6.1, Variety of waveforms in neuromodulation. Some of these have been already used commercially, while others are experimental variants. These represent examples of the infinite number of possibilities which could be tried.

The amount of energy delivered to the tissues can be determined as a multiplication of frequency × pulsewidth × amplitude, for a given impedance, per second. This value in and of itself, however, neglects several aspects of how neural tissues respond to various waveforms with their own nonlinear dynamics to the timing and other features of the energy delivery. A simple illustration of this can be shown in work from Rall [ ] ( Fig. 6.2 here), studying dendritic processing in a passive cable model of dendrites and current injection at several locations along the dendrite from distal to the axon hillock and cell body in general and closer to it. If one uses the same total energy in each case, but in the first instance, injects a pulse of current from distal moving closer to the cell body in order, or in the second instance starting with the first pulse at the cell body and then moving distally, it becomes obvious that what the axon hillock “sees” over time, and which is the determinant largely of whether the neuron will fire an AP itself or not, is a completely different rise and fall of current in both cases, though their total energy delivered was the same. Extrapolating to this discussion, it is therefore not a foregone conclusion that 100 Hz, delivered with a 50 us pulsewidth at 2 mA is going to do the same thing as 50 Hz delivered with a 200 uS pulsewidth at 1 mA, or to a 5 kHz stimulus at 10 uS pulsewidth delivered at 0.2 mA for that matter. All of these waveforms, albeit all using steadily produced square waves without pauses between the anode and cathode phases, will likely have grossly different effects on an axon, though they use the same total energy delivery. Additionally, not all axons are the same, some are myelinated, some not, some large in diameter others smaller, and almost certainly they will each have slightly or radically different membrane parameters.

Figure 6.2, From Rall ( Fig. 5 , 1964)—This shows how very different the resulting current as seen at the axon hillock would be (here only for a passive cable model, but would be even less similar most likely if more details nonlinear cable features were included), if the same overall energy were delivered to the neuron. In the dendritic sequence in the upper left , epsp's allow current flux into the dendritic cable from closer to the cell body initially, then from progressively further distal in time. The current arrives at the axon hillock sooner, but decays sooner as well and so adds in a way that has a faster peak, but lower overall and somewhat more evenly sustained. This is in contrast to the same total energy imparted in opposite sequence, from distal to proximal. The peak is delayed but higher in amplitude.

Most spinal cord stimulation has, until very recently, used lower frequency (<200 Hz) charge-balanced square waves, with pulsewidths in the range of 50–300 uS, and amplitudes typically within the range of 1–5 mA. Most systems now deliver “constant-current” stimulation, based on the intercontact impedances (which doesn't necessarily give any accurate information on what actually reaches the spinal cord), and don't make instantaneous adjustments to account for movement of the spinal cord itself, which is known to be occurring every few milliseconds with breathing, heartbeat, and physical movement and posture. This factor will be discussed further below.

It is also important to understand the efficiency for nerve fiber activation using various waveform “shapes.” This has been studied in detail (see Refs. [ , ]). The long-standing hypothesis that a rising exponential waveform shape was most energy efficient was refuted and, given the variations in cell dynamics, pulsewidth, and waveform shape, no shape was found to be simultaneously energy, charge, and power-optimal. The main factor in refuting this hypothesis turned out to be that prior work had analyzed linear membrane models for charge integration. Nonlinear models which took into account that the threshold rises with stimulation (accommodation) gave a more realistic and sophisticated view of how neurons actually respond. Not all neurons integrate current with the same set of membrane parameters or have the same degree of threshold accommodation. Moreover, spatial aspects of fiber location relative to the current source contribute to waveform shape efficiency as well, such that axon orientation, intervening tissue characteristics, and distance all can alter what shape may be most efficient for a particular circumstance. More detailed analysis of waveform shape and efficiency under various conditions and using different techniques has been performed and is further reviewed concisely in Ref. [ ].

You're Reading a Preview

Become a Clinical Tree membership for Full access and enjoy Unlimited articles

Become membership

If you are a member. Log in here