Neurodevelopmental Follow-Up of High-Risk Newborns


INTRODUCTION

Though progress in newborn medicine since the 1960s has led to the dramatic increase of survival in treated patients, it may be offset by an increase in neurodevelopmental impairments in these patients. Moreover, these developmental issues are not always evident in the neonatal period but appear throughout the first year of life as neuromotor deficits or later in childhood as cognitive, educational, and socioemotional deficits. The term “neurodevelopmental outcome” appeared for the first time around 1976 when neonatologists began to systematically follow up high-risk newborns from their neonatal intensive care units (NICUs) and to document their neurological development throughout the first years of life and into childhood. These studies were based on earlier findings on normal developing children by Gesell and Ilg in the Yale Clinic of Child Development and by the Zurich longitudinal child development studies. To date, Medline lists around 12,000 research publications using the term “neurodevelopmental outcome,” with the number increasing yearly such that the number of publications has tripled since the last edition of this chapter. When new treatments are introduced in newborn medicine, neurodevelopmental outcome is now considered a primary endpoint, alongside survival and neonatal morbidity. Neonatal care does not cease on discharge from the nursery, and as a field we have ongoing responsibility to monitor and support the development of NICU graduates.

There is irrefutable evidence that critically ill neonates, such as those born very preterm or neonates with conditions affecting the brain (e.g., hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy, congenital heart disease, intrauterine growth restriction), are at risk for long-term neurodevelopmental impairments. Areas of concern include sensory deficits, cognitive and learning difficulties, motor problems, and emotional-behavioral issues, which collectively can limit educational, vocational, social, and participatory opportunities. Neurodevelopmental concerns can present early and persist throughout life, or they may improve or worsen with increasing age. Some areas of concern may not become obvious until later in development when those skills emerge and are observable, such as high-level language and cognitive skills. If deficits are subtle, functional problems may not surface until children are faced with complex scenarios that challenge those abilities, such as during the transition to adolescence and the later years of school. Furthermore, there is marked individual heterogeneity with regards to the severity and nature of long-term impairments, even among infants with similar clinical journeys. Though there are numerous clinical and social factors that are associated with long-term outcomes, it remains difficult to predict which children will present with future challenges and the nature of those challenges.

Critically ill neonates warrant close and regular surveillance throughout childhood, given their high rate of neurodevelopmental impairments and the resources implemented in the neonatal period for their survival. Indeed, it is now widely accepted that these infants require such monitoring as part of the ongoing responsibility for care and optimization of their outcomes beyond merely survival and discharge from the NICU. Though surveillance in the first 2 to 3 years of life is typical for these children, less common is neurodevelopmental monitoring in the preschool period and beyond. Limiting surveillance to the first few years is inadequate because many developmental problems do not surface until later in life, and problems identified in infancy can appear to resolve with maturity but, in reality, evolve and present differently. Thus a challenge when assessing high-risk infants is determining whether a problem detected in infancy reflects a developmental delay or a persisting impairment. Although risk factors may assist with this differentiation, ongoing monitoring is ultimately needed to track development for signs of catch-up. So though standardized assessments of developmental status such as the Bayley Scales are important for detecting current strengths and weakness, these assessments are only moderately predictive of later functioning.

PRIMARY PURPOSE OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL FOLLOW-UP

The primary purpose of neurodevelopmental follow-up for high-risk infants is to detect developmental problems as early as possible . This enables interventions to be implemented to support the child during a sensitive period of development and to maximize the chances of preventing or minimizing long-term functional consequences. Otherwise, if left unmanaged, early developmental problems can worsen, become pervasive and very difficult to treat, and ultimately lead to significant and complex impairments, which also present huge burdens on families and society. Early intervention is a key strategy for optimizing long-term developmental outcomes with concomitant effects on societal wealth and preservation of human capital. However, targeted remediation is dependent on the early detection of areas of concern.

Neurodevelopmental follow-up is a high priority for families of high-risk infants. Families are likely to be informed that their child has a higher than average chance for developmental problems and, accordingly, many seek regular feedback on the health and development of their child and what to expect in the future. Such information is important for managing parental anxiety and expectations, educating parents to monitor for signs for potential emerging problems, and family decision-making. It enables families to proactively plan and advocate for their child’s needs. Such planning and advocacy can be especially relevant at key developmental transition points when additional challenges may be encountered by a child, such as starting child care or changing schools. During these times, families are also likely to benefit from extra advice and support.

Many neurodevelopmental follow-up programs collect core data that can be used for clinical audits or research . Given constant advances in obstetrical and neonatal management, it is essential that data be collected on the development of high-risk infants to assess the effectiveness of ongoing changes in care. Neonatal clinicians should have a good understanding of this information, because it is relevant for clinical decision-making and counseling families. Unfortunately, this understanding often is not the case, with research demonstrating that clinicians tend to overestimate major disability in infants born extremely preterm. In the context of randomized controlled trials, efficacy of neonatal interventions should be judged according to long-term developmental outcomes, in addition to survival rates and neonatal morbidity, because some interventions may have adverse neurological consequences that are not obvious until middle childhood, adolescence, or even early adulthood. Data from follow-up programs can also be used for benchmarking neonatal services, with developmental outcomes for high-risk infants compared at local, regional, and national levels.

In summary, neurodevelopmental follow-up of high-risk infants is important for providing ongoing clinical support for the child and the family, with the aim of optimizing long-term health and development and reducing dependency on health, educational, and social services .

NEURODEVELOPMENT

To accurately identify deviations and delays in a child’s development, it is essential to have an appreciation of the normative developmental timing of different skills and abilities, as well as the processes that shape child development, which consist of physiological adaptations and disruptions, cumulative over time and embedded in sensitive periods. Factors compromising child development are both biological and contextual. Important biological risk factors are linked to the prenatal environment, such as intrauterine factors that lead to intrauterine growth restrictions and preterm birth and complications associated with these conditions, such as perinatal brain injury, as well as a lack of adequate nutrition and infections. Equally important are contextual risk factors , such as environmental toxins, drugs in the prenatal period, parental psychological health (depression, stress, anxiety), suboptimal early care environment, and low educational level ( Fig. 14.1 ).

Fig. 14.1, Factors influencing child development.

Life Course Development

Child development is, by definition, dynamic over time , with both quantitative and qualitative changes in brain structure and functioning observed from birth to adulthood ( Fig. 14.2 ). Neurodevelopment commences early in fetal development, driven by genetic programs that are influenced by, and often dependent on, environmental experiences. For example, early perinatal experiences have the potential to both positively or negatively alter an individual’s life course trajectory, and a child’s family, school, and community experiences will mold his or her neurodevelopment and functional abilities in distinct ways. Given that early life experiences can affect an individual’s development over his or her life course, the term life course development is often used . These life experiences are unique for each individual, which helps to explain the vast interindividual variability in long-term outcomes for high-risk infants. Another challenge when assessing young children is the wide variations in rate in which skills emerge and mature ( Fig. 14.3 ). These dynamic changes across development and the critical influence of environmental exposures throughout the life span ( Fig. 14.4 ) can make pediatric prediction of diagnosis and prognosis difficult and unreliable. For this reason, ongoing monitoring of development across the life span and the collection of information from multiple sources are advised for high-risk infants. Thus long-term follow-up enables the reliable assessment of skills that do not become apparent until school age or later, as well as the determination of whether an early developmental deviation reflects a delay or ongoing impairment.

Fig. 14.2, Brain development and the association with key functional abilities.

Fig. 14.3, Protracted development of primary neurodevelopmental domains.

Fig. 14.4, Development of key functional abilities are largely influenced by environment, especially during sensitive periods of development.

Domains of Development

Neurodevelopment is a broad concept, consisting of distinct yet interdependent domains. Broadly, a child’s neurodevelopment can be conceptualized into domains of (1) physical health/growth, (2) neuromotor function, (3) language, (4) cognition and learning, (5) emotional and behavioral well-being and mental health, and (6) social functioning. Each domain comprises a set of skills. For example, the neuromotor function domain includes reflexes, balance, coordination, and fine motor dexterity, and the cognition and learning domain includes general intelligence (intelligence quotient [IQ]), attention, memory, executive functioning, literacy, and numeracy. Though these neurodevelopmental domains are usually assessed separately, and often by different health professionals (e.g., pediatrician, psychologist, physiotherapist, speech pathologist), they should be interpreted in the context of the other domains because functioning in one domain can have an adverse effect on developmental opportunities and functioning in another. Relatedly, early skills acquired during infancy and early childhood form vital building blocks for later competencies not only within the same domain but also across domains. For example, a child with delayed language development may be more prone to develop regulatory and behavior problems because of his or her difficulties in communicating needs to others. Similarly, a child with cerebral palsy (CP) who is unable to participate fully in sports at school may have fewer opportunities to interact and develop friendships with his or her peers, which in turn may limit social learning experiences and long-term social development. These cascading challenges can have adverse cumulative effects on a child’s functioning over time.

Importance of a Child’s Postnatal Experiences

Though a child’s functional capacity is an expression of his or her genetic inheritance and the associated maturation of brain structures and networks, these processes are the product of complex interactions with environmental influences. Specifically, an individual’s genes provide essential programs for establishing basic patterns of neuronal growth and connectivity, but the environmental experiences during sensitive periods of development influence how the genes are expressed (epigenetics) and accordingly shape the developing brain architecture and functioning (see Chapters 7 and 8 ). Both the establishment and consolidation of brain networks during sensitive periods are dependent on brain activity; that is, extrinsic sensory input is needed for the development of normal sensory development.

Factors that help optimize brain development include (1) responsive, emotionally supportive, and developmentally stimulating interactions; (2) protection from environmental toxins as well as physical and psychological threats; and (3) access to good nutrition, health care, and early learning opportunities. Unfortunately, these factors are not part of the environment for all children, such as those exposed to varying degrees of environmental risk, including neglect, poverty, social adversity, poor parental mental health, parenting problems, family dysfunction, and neighborhood/community stress or violence. Evidence shows that these adverse experiences, which often co-occur, adversely affect brain development and, in turn, a child’s physical, cognitive, and social development. Furthermore, these risk factors may be additive or even interactive , such that the severity of developmental problems increases with exposure to multiple risk factors. High-risk infants may be even more sensitive to these experiential risks than typically developing children, gaining greater benefit from an optimal rearing environment and potentially being more severely affected by exposure to an adverse rearing environment.

The child is obviously an active participant in his or her world, with the relationship between a child and his or her environment being interactional. For example, a child who is behaviorally difficult or is more demanding to care for developmentally can put strain on family members and family relationships, increasing the risks of maternal stress/depression as well as marital discord. These parental and family challenges in turn can impede the family’s ability to parent and manage difficult child behavior, further exacerbating problems. Thus though environmental factors are important in shaping child outcomes, the child’s characteristics partially influence the nature of his or her own environment.

WHICH INFANTS NEED FOLLOW-UP

Numerous factors increase an infant’s risk for later developmental impairments ( Table 14.1 ) . A workshop on the follow-up of high-risk infants sponsored by the National Institute of Child Health and Development and the National Institute of Neurological Diseases and Stroke identified a wide range of biological, interventional, and socioenvironmental factors for infants born preterm and term that are associated with long-term impairments and warrant ongoing monitoring. More recently, an Australasian consortium that proposed a framework for specialized follow-up of high-risk infants used broader criteria to determine an infant’s risk and need for surveillance, including preterm birth, congenital disorders, birth asphyxia, infection, and family disadvantages.

TABLE 14.1
Child Health and Family Factors That Indicate the Need for Neurodevelopmental Follow-Up
CHILD FACTORS FAMILY FACTORS
  • VPT: gestational age <32 weeks or very low birth weight (VLBW: <1500 g)

  • Small for gestational age (SGA: <3rd percentile or <–2 SD weight for gestational age and sex), microcephaly, IUGR with pathological Doppler

  • Neonatal asphyxia and/or neonatal encephalopathy (including seizure), regardless of cause

  • Term babies who have received positive pressure ventilation for >24 hours

  • Congenital brain or heart malformations, genetic syndromes, or inborn errors of metabolism that affect neurodevelopmental outcomes

  • Congenital infections

  • Neonatal abstinence syndrome

  • Failed newborn hearing screening

  • Neonatal central nervous system infections (e.g., meningitis/encephalitis)

  • Infants requiring major surgery (e.g., brain, cardiac, other thoracic, or abdominal)

  • Hyperbilirubinemia (bilirubin >400 µmol/L or clinical evidence of bilirubin encephalopathy)

  • Neurobehavioral abnormalities noted in the newborn period

  • High social risk (e.g., interpartner violence, previous child abuse, severe poverty, or homelessness)

  • Substance abuse by either parent

  • Major psychiatric history in either parent

  • Developmental disability in either parent

IUGR , Intrauterine growth restriction; SGA , small for gestational age; VPT , very preterm.
From Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, Battin M, et al. Long term follow up of high risk children: who, why and how? BMC Pediatr . 2014;14:279.

Infants exposed to multiple risk factors may be considered at even greater risk for developmental problems . For example, a growth curve analysis of the cognitive trajectories of very preterm between the ages of 4 and 12 years found that neonatal cerebral white matter abnormalities (WMAs) and family social risk (early motherhood, low maternal education, minority ethnicity, low socioeconomic status, and single-parent family) contributed additively to children’s later cognitive outcome. Children with no WMAs and no family social risk fared the best, with the highest average cognitive trajectory. The second highest functioning group, performing on average 4.7 points lower, were those children who had no observable WMAs on term magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) but were raised in high social risk environments (more than social risk factors), reflecting the developmental effect associated with raising a child in a socioeconomically disadvantaged family environment. Children with neonatal WMAs but who were raised in a family environment unaffected by social and economic adversity obtained an average IQ score that was 7.4 IQ points lower than the lowest risk preterm group, whereas those with both risk factors (i.e., moderate to severe WMAs and high social risk) performed an average of 12 IQ points lower. Thus both medical and contextual factors are important in determining a child’s level of risk . It is also relevant to note that children from disadvantaged sociodemographic backgrounds are less likely to attend developmental follow-up, despite being at greater risk.

MODELS OF NEURODEVELOPMENTAL FOLLOW-UP

Despite the benefits of surveillance for high-risk infants, most institutions do not have the resources to closely monitor the development of all high-risk infants beyond the first couple of months posthospital discharge. Consequently, individual centers/regions usually target specific subgroups of children to follow up, with the timing and structure of follow-up dependent on the resources available. Broadly, surveillance models can incorporate (1) formal surveillance by neonatal follow-up specialists, (2) screening followed by formal surveillance by neonatal follow-up specialists when indicated, or (3) screening followed by referral to specialists in the community when indicated ( Fig. 14.5 ).

Fig. 14.5, Different models of neurodevelopmental follow-up.

Surveillance by neonatal follow-up specialists (model 1) is typically reserved for the highest risk infants, because this model involves assessments by a multidisciplinary team (e.g., pediatrics, psychology, speech pathology, physiotherapy, occupational therapy) and is resource intensive. The structure of these follow-up programs varies depending on the age of the child and training of the follow-up specialists but should provide a comprehensive analysis of the child’s current strengths and challenges. Depending on the nature and severity of the challenges, children may be referred for more intensive evaluation or intervention services. Even though specialized neonatal follow-up is recommended for all high-risk groups outlined in Table 14.1 , due to limited resources it is often the case that only specific subgroups of infants can access these services. Well-resourced families may be able to organize multidisciplinary assessments themselves within the public health system or privately, but the specialists are less likely to have the same level of expertise with NICU survivors.

Models 2 and 3 commence with screening, comprising a parent interview and/or standardized questionnaires (e.g., Ages and Stages Questionnaire) designed to detect children experiencing developmental issues or at high-risk for emerging difficulties. Children qualify for a referral to a specialized neonatal follow-up program if screened to be at high-risk (model 2) or, alternatively, referred to specialist services in the child’s community (model 3). Models of care that incorporate screening can monitor the developmental status for a much larger range of neonatal conditions and a larger number of high-risk infants. These models aim to reserve limited health resources for those children who warrant surveillance (i.e., those who are experiencing developmental concerns), rather than targeting a specific population, some of whom will be developing well and not in need for formal specialist follow-up. However, screening is not perfect. It is typically based on parental perceptions of their child’s development, and some children will screen incorrectly as developing age appropriately and not warranting formal follow-up services, whereas others will screen incorrectly as being of high-risk and referred to specialist services that they do not need. Furthermore, the process for categorizing a child as being at high risk at screening has important implications; if the bar is set too high, a significant proportion of needy children will not be referred to specialist services, whereas if the bar is set too low, a significant proportion will be referred who do not need it.

TIMING OF FOLLOW-UP ASSESSMENTS

Following discharge from neonatal intensive care, infants will typically be monitored by either a neonatal specialist or pediatrician in the subsequent 3 to 12 months and then referred to a developmental specialist according to needs or existence of a formal follow-up program. Table 14.2 provides a list of recommended ages for follow-up and the relevant child outcomes and family factors to assess, based on expert opinion during an Australasian workshop. In general, it was recommended that high-risk infants be assessed regularly in the first 2 years (intervals of not more than 6 months) and then annually to school entry if the child is progressing well. For school-aged children (5 to 18 years), the expert workshop recommended less frequent follow-up with a focus on higher order cognitive functioning, academic progress, emotional and behavioral status, and social functioning. Throughout this period, additional visits may be indicated with the presence or emergence of specific medical or neurodevelopmental issues. The transition to adolescence and then adulthood may be associated with the emergence of new medical, cognitive, and mental health challenges and, as such, follow-up during these periods can be warranted. Table 14.2 also demonstrates that the neurodevelopmental domains of interest vary across recommended follow-up time points, reflecting the child’s changing developmental status and milestones, the emergence of specific abilities, and periods in which abilities are functionally important.

TABLE 14.2
Child and Family Outcomes to Be Considered at Different Ages
2–6 3–4 8 12 15–18 24 36 4–5 6–8 12–14 TRANSITION
WEEKS MONTHS MONTHS MONTHS MONTHS MONTHS MONTHS YEARS a YEARS b YEARS TO ADULT ADULT
Child
Physical Health
General health +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++
Growth +++ +++ +++ +++ ++ ++ ++ ++ ++ +++ c ++ d ++ d
Feeding problems +++ ++ ++ ++ + + + 0 0 0 0 0
Special senses +++ ++ ++ + + + + + + + + +
Neurological +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ ++ ++ + + + +
Motor skills + ++ ++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ ++
Blood pressure/ UR UR UR UR UR +/− +/− ++ +++ +++ +++ +++
CVS
Respiratory health +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ ++ ++ +++ +++ +++ +++
Daily functioning ++ ++ ++ ++ ++ ++ +++ +++ +-H- +++ +++ +++
Learning and cognition
Development/cognitive function ++ ++ ++ ++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ ++ ++
Language + ++ +++ e +++ e +++ e +++ e +++ +++ +++ + 0 0
Preacademic skills 0 0 0 0 0 0 + +++ ++ 0 0 0
Academic 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 +++ +++ +++ ++ f
progress
Mental Health
Behavior +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++
Social skills + + ++ e +++ e +++ e +++ e +++ e +++ +++ +++ +++ +++
Psychopathology 0 0 0 + e + e ++ e +++e ++ +++ +++ +++ +++
Risk-taking 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 ++ +++ +++
Family
Parental mental health +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++
Caregiver-child interaction +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ ++ + + + 0
Family function +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++
Siblings +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++ +++
0 , Does not apply; + to +++ , reflects relative importance; +/− , of dubious value; CVS , cardiovascular system; UR , unreliable.
Shaded areas represent a suggested minimal checklist for busy clinicians.
From Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, Battin M, et al. Long term follow up of high risk children: who, why and how? BMC Pediatr . 2014;14:279.

a Prior to school entry.

b 1–2 years after starting school.

c Growth at 12–14 years includes normal pubertal development.

d Overweight/obesity an ongoing issue.

e Relevant to early presentation of autism spectrum disorder.

f Ongoing life learning.

The follow-up framework from the Australasian workshop is outlined in Table 14.2 and was proposed as a guide for surveillance programs when structuring the timing of their follow-up and the domains to target. The National Institute for Health and Care Excellence (NICE) guidelines for the developmental follow-up of children and young people born preterm provide a pragmatic model of care. Though the assessment tools recommended in the NICE guidelines are widely available and easy to adopt for follow-up programs, they are broad measures, and more specialized evaluations will often be required. The European Standards of Care for Newborn Health provide a well-balanced framework that provides flexibility in implementation.

It is common for specialist neonatal follow-up programs to be funded to provide surveillance for only the first 24 to 36 months, although in some regions and centers there is a gradual trend to extend programs into the early school years. As already noted, this duration is inadequate, and follow-up for high-risk infants should continue to adolescence at least, because early developmental assessments are only moderately predictive of later outcomes, many domains of concern do not emerge or are not assessable until school-age or beyond, the nature of impairments can evolve and manifest differently with increasing age, and subtle but important problems may not surface until appropriately challenging scenarios are confronted.

For children born very preterm, it is important that their age be adjusted for the degree of prematurity for at least the first 24 to 36 months. Research demonstrates that children will be undeservedly disadvantaged on cognitive test measures without age correction, and adjusted developmental levels better predict future neurodevelopmental performance. This bias is most marked during early childhood but does persist to a lesser degree into school age. However, it needs to be acknowledged that age correction for prematurity may have implications for a child being eligible for much-needed interventions and services; for example, a child may meet criteria for early intervention services based on assessment results using chronological age but is not eligible when applying age correction, which increases assessment scores. Thus the decision to apply age correction beyond age 2 years is largely dependent on the purpose of the assessment and the needs of the child.

NEURODEVELOPMENTAL FOLLOW-UP PROGRAMS

Follow-up teams are typically multidisciplinary, reflecting the range of domains that need to be assessed. The composition of the team may vary across the follow-up time points to cater for the specific domains of interest and the measures to be administered. Programs that monitor development from infancy to school age may include pediatricians, neonatologists, developmental and behavioral pediatricians, child neurologists, developmental and clinical psychologists, neuropsychologists, physiotherapists, specialists in psychomotor development, occupational therapists, and speech pathologists.

A formal evaluation of a child’s developmental status and functioning is typically undertaken using standardized assessments of the child, some of which are discipline specific and require accredited training, as well as standardized parent questionnaires. Direct observation also provides invaluable information. Though there are instruments that rate observations of the child, qualitative features of the child’s developmental capacities and behaviors are more commonly done informally by experienced health professionals during natural interactions in the waiting room and during the assessment.

In addition to deciding on the developmental domains to assess, follow-up teams should select which standardized assessments to be used . The NICE guidelines, as well as the Adults born Preterm International Collaboration, propose common core measures for assessing young children and adults born preterm, but teams are likely to make decisions based on personnel training and expertise, as well as regional preferences and requirements. For most domains a range of standardized measures are available, and selection should take into account psychometric properties, including construct validity and test-retest reliability, as well as inter- and intratester reliability. One of the most important considerations should be how the measures are standardized. The tests should have norms that are based on age brackets that are relevant to the age range of the children attending the follow-up program, and the norms should be generalizable to the local population. For example, follow-up programs in the United States should use tests that have U.S. norms, whereas programs in the UK should prioritize tests with UK norms. Furthermore, preference should be given to the most recent revisions of tests, because norms shift with time and can be out of date if they are 10 to 20 years old. Administration time is an important pragmatic issue when selecting tests, and it is not uncommon for teams to select abbreviated measures so that a broad range of domains can be assessed in the scheduled appointment time.

Measurement error is an issue that needs to be acknowledged when assessing development and, as such, interpreting performance should consider the 95% confidence interval of test scores, qualitative observations, and feedback from parents. Measurement bias can also occur. Tester bias occurs when preassessment information of the child influences how the evaluator administers measures and interprets test performance. For example, an examiner may decide not to administer certain items or tests to a child he or she was told had a specific impairment, despite the child being able to complete some of the tasks. In assessments conducted for research purposes, it is critical that evaluators be blinded to details of the child that are relevant to the study (e.g., group membership, intervention arm) to minimize tester bias or the perception of tester bias. Another measurement bias, which is particularly common for behavioral questionnaires, is informant bias . Questionnaires are generally completed by a single person, usually the child’s primary caregiver, and their responses reflect their perceptions, which may differ from those of another respondent (e.g., teacher). For all respondents, perceptions can be influenced by their personality, past experiences, and mental health status. As such, there are numerous reports of significant discrepancies between parent and teacher responses and parent and self-informant responses. Ideally, perspectives from multiple informants will be sought, but this is often not practical.

Well-functioning follow-up programs have significant administrative support , which is needed to contact families, confirm appointments, and maintain communication. Families move, change contact details, and may not be motivated to attend the program. As a result, all programs have some degree of family withdrawal, noncompliance, or nonattendance. A point of concern is that children and families with developmental challenges and/or from socioeconomically disadvantaged backgrounds are overrepresented in this group who do not receive surveillance or are more difficult to engage. Families can be prepared for follow-up appointments during the hospitalization period, and early follow-up appointments can be organized prior to discharge. Some family circumstances make it challenging for families to attend appointments, in which case alternative arrangements should be considered, such as home visits, if possible, or referral to other services that increase the ease for families to attend.

FUNCTIONAL NEURODEVELOPMENTAL DOMAINS

Functional neurodevelopmental domains that should form part of a comprehensive follow-up program include (1) physical health/growth , (2) neuromotor function , (3) language , (4) cognition and learning , (5) emotional and behavioral well-being or mental health , and (6) social functioning . Details relating to each of these domains are discussed next and summarized in Table 14.3 , with an emphasis on those most pertinent to the follow-up of high-risk infants. This section will highlight relevant variables that neonatal follow-up programs should consider evaluating and how these may change in importance with the increasing age of the child. The focus of the follow-up in terms of the domains assessed and method of assessment will vary at different follow-up time points and will differ across follow-up programs to account for the purpose, resources, and time available for each individual program.

TABLE 14.3
Commonly Used Measures in Neurodevelopmental Follow-Up
TEST AGE PRIMARY SCALE(S)
Developmental Screening
Ages & Stage Questionnaire 1 month to 5.5 years Communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem solving, personal-social
Denver Developmental Screening Test Birth to 6 years Fine motor, gross motor, language, personal-social
Parent Report of Children’s Abilities–Revised 24 months Cognition and language
Motor Skills
Prechtl’s General Movement Assessment 0–20 weeks corrected Gross movements, writhing movements, fidgety movements
AIMS 0–18 months Prone, Supine, Sitting, Standing
TIMP 34 weeks’ gestation to 4 months corrected Postural and selective control of movement needed for functional motor performance
NSMDA 1 month–6 years Gross Motor, Fine Motor, Neurological, Primitive Reflexes, Postural Reactions, Sensorimotor Response
Bayley-4 1–42 months Fine Motor, Gross Motor, Motor (Composite)
Movement Assessment Battery for Children 3–16 years Manual Dexterity, Ball Skills, Static and Dynamic Balance
Gross Motor Function Classification System <2–18 years Walks without limitations, walks with limitations, walks using a handheld mobility device, self-mobility with limitations/may use powered mobility, transported in a manual wheelchair
Language Skills
Bayley-4 1–42 months Receptive Language, Expressive Language, Language (Composite)
MacArthur-Bates Communication Development Inventories 8–37 months Words and Gestures, Words and Sentences, Expressive Vocabulary and Grammar
Preschool Language Scales 0–7 years Total Language, Auditory Comprehension, Expressive Communication
Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamental–Preschool 3–6 years Core Language Score, Receptive Language, Expressive Language, Language Content, Language Structure
Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamental 5–21 years Core Language Score, Receptive Language, Expressive Language, Language Structure, Language Content, Language Memory, Working Memory
General Cognition
Bayley-4 1–42 months Cognitive Development
Griffith’s Mental Development Scales 1 month –5 years Locomotor, Personal-Social, Language, Eye and Hand Coordination, Performance, Practical Reasoning
Mullen Scales of Early Learning 0–68 months Early Learning Composite, Visual Reception, Expressive Language, Receptive Language
Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence 2 years, 6 months – 7 years, 7 months Full-scale IQ, Verbal Comprehension, Visual Spatial, Fluid Reasoning, Working Memory, Processing Speed
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children 6–16 years Full-scale IQ, Verbal Comprehension, Visual Spatial, Working Memory, Fluid Reasoning, Processing Speed
Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales 2–85+ years Full-scale IQ, Verbal IQ, Nonverbal IQ, Fluid Reasoning, Knowledge, Quantitative Reasoning, Visual-Spatial Processing, Working Memory
Differential Ability Scales 2 years, 6 months –17 years, 11 years General Conceptual Ability, Verbal, nonverbal, and spatial reasoning
Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children 3–18 years Fluid-Crystallized, Simultaneous, Sequential, Planning, Learning, Knowledge
Wechsler Adult Scale of Intelligence 16–91 years Full-scale IQ, Verbal Comprehension, Perceptual Reasoning, Working Memory, Processing Speed
Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence 6–91 years Full-scale IQ, Verbal Comprehension, Perceptual Reasoning
Attention
TEACh 6–15 years Selective Attention, Sustained Attention, Attentional Switching, Verbal Working Memory
Conners 3 ADHD Index 6–18 years ADHD Index
ADHD Rating Scale-5 5–17 years ADHD, Inattention, Hyperactivity-Impulsivity
Brown Attention-Deficit Disorder Scales 3 years and older Organizing, prioritizing & activating to work; Focusing, sustaining & shifting attention to tasks; Regulating alertness, sustaining effort & processing speed; Managing frustration & modulating emotions; Utilizing working memory & accessing recall; Monitoring & self-regulating action
Strengths and Weaknesses of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity-symptoms and Normal-behaviors rating scale < 18 years Inattention, Hyperactivity-Impulsivity
Memory and Learning
California Verbal Learning Test 5–16 years Attention Span, Learning Efficiency, Free and Cued Recall, Interference
Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test 7 years and older Attention Span, Learning, Interference, Delayed Recall
Children’s Memory Scale 5–16 years Verbal Immediate, Verbal Delayed, Visual Immediate, Visual Delayed, General Memory, Delayed Recognition, Learning, Attention/Concentration
Wide Range Assessment of Memory and Learning 5–90 years Verbal Memory, Visual Memory, Attention/Concentration
Test of Memory and Learning 5–60 years Verbal Memory, Nonverbal Memory, Composite Memory Index, Verbal Delayed Recall, Learning, Attention/Concentration
Executive Functioning
EF Touch 3–6 years Inhibitory control, working memory attentional shifting
A Developmental Neuropsychological Assessment 3–16 years Executive Function and Attention, Language, Memory & Learning, Sensorimotor, Visuospatial Processing, Social Perception
Delis–Kaplan Executive Function System 8–89 years Trail-Making, Verbal Fluency, Design Fluency, Color-Word Interference, Sorting, Twenty Questions, Word Context, Tower, Proverb Tests
BRIEF2 Preschool version: 2–6 years; School-aged version: 5–18 years Behavior Regulation, Emotion Regulation, Cognitive Regulation, Inhibit, Shift, Emotional Control, Initiate, Working Memory, Plan/Organize, Organization of Materials, Monitor
Academic Progress
Wechsler Individual Achievement Test 4–50 years Total Achievement Composite, Reading Composite, Written Expression Composite, Mathematics Composite
Wide Range Achievement Test 5–85 years Word Reading, Sentence Comprehension, Spelling, Math Computation, Reading Composite
Woodcock-Johnson Tests of Achievement 2–90 years Achievement Score, Reading, Math, Writing, Cross-Domain
Child Behavior
Infant-Toddler Social and Emotional Assessment 1–3 years Externalizing behaviors, Internalizing behaviors, Dysregulation, Maladaptive behaviors, Competence
Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire 2–17 years Total difficulties, Emotional symptoms, Conduct problems, Hyperactivity/inattention, Peer relationship problems, Prosocial behavior
Child Behavior Checklist 1.5–18 years Internalizing Problems, Externalizing Problems, Total Behavior Problems. Syndrome Scales, DSM-Oriented Scales
Behavioral Assessment System for Children 2–21 years Behavioral Symptom Index, Externalizing Problems, Internalizing Problems, School Problems, Adaptive Skills
Mental Health
Development and Well-Being Assessment (5–17 years) 5–17 years Structured interview: ICD-10, DSM-IV, DSM-5 psychiatric diagnoses
Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children and Adolescents < 18 years Structured interview: ICD-10, DSM-IV psychiatric diagnoses
Diagnostic Interview for Children and Adolescents 6–17 years; 3–7 years Structured interview: DSM-IV psychiatric disorders
Children’s Interview for Psychiatric Syndromes 6–18 years Structured interview: DSM-IV psychiatric disorders
Modified Checklist for Autism in Toddlers 16–30 months ASD screen
Social Communication Questionnaire 4 years and older ASD screen
Social Responsiveness Scale 2.5 years and older ASD screen: Social Awareness, Social Cognition, Social Communication, Social Motivation, Restricted Interests/Repetitive Behavior
Gilliam Autism Rating Scale 3–22 years ASD screen: Restricted Interests/Repetitive Behavior, Social Interaction, Social Communication, Emotional Responses, Cognitive Style, Maladaptive Speech
Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule 12 months and older Semistructured, standardized assessment of ASD: Communication, Social Interaction, Play, Restricted & Repetitive Behavior
Daily Functioning
Adaptive Behavior Assessment System Birth and older Communication, Community Use, Functional Academics, Health & Safety, Home/School Living, Leisure, Self-Care, Self-Direction, Social, Work, Motor
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales Birth and older Communication, Daily Living Skills, Socialization, Motor Skills
Scales of Independent Behavior Infancy and older Motor Skills, Social Interaction & Communication Skills, Personal Living Skills, Community Living Skills
Parental Mental Health
Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale Adulthood Postnatal depression
Center for Epidemiological Studies Depression Scale Adulthood Depression symptoms
Generalized Anxiety Disorder Questionnaire Adulthood Anxiety symptoms

Child Characteristics

Physical Health/Growth

This domain includes a number of different areas of potential health concern.

You're Reading a Preview

Become a Clinical Tree membership for Full access and enjoy Unlimited articles

Become membership

If you are a member. Log in here