Avoiding Patient Harm in Anesthesia: Human Performance and Patient Safety


Key Points

  • Excellent clinical performance is not achieved by the use of sound medical knowledge alone, as clinicians have to face multifaceted challenges not just medical issues. There is an increased awareness that human factors—both on the individual and the team level—as well as organizational factors in the health care system play major roles in providing excellent medical care. Therefore, for anesthesia professionals (1) the study of human performance is fundamental, (2) the knowledge and successful application of efficient safety strategies are highly relevant, and (3) understanding of the pertinent organizational matters is very important.

  • The health care system in general, individual clinical institutions, and work units in particular must provide the appropriate organizational characteristics to facilitate safe patient care including: promoting a culture of safety, effective incident reporting and analysis systems, continuous training for professionals, and optimized structures and processes.

  • Organizations that provide such characteristics are referred to as high-reliability organizations (HRO). HRO theory describes the key features of systems that conduct complex and hazardous work, but do so with extremely low failure rates and complications. The credo of HROs is not to erase all errors but rather to identify human error mechanisms and make systems more impervious to errors and their sequelae (resilience).

  • Several mechanisms of optimal versus poor performance have been demonstrated through human performance research. A particular technique of human performance research called task analysis has been useful in understanding the work of anesthesia professionals. Observing them during routine operations or in the handling of (simulated) adverse events has improved our knowledge on human performance. The findings include the impact of critical and continuous situation awareness and decision making (i.e., core cognitive process model), effective teamwork, leadership, and communication, as well as task management and the use of cognitive aids (e.g., checklists or emergency manuals).

  • Organizations and individuals need to fully recognize that the performance of individual anesthesia professionals can—as for all human beings—be adversely influenced by performance-shaping factors, including noise, illness, aging, boredom, distraction, sleep deprivation, and fatigue, as well as by social dynamics within and between crews and teams.

  • It is necessary to have a clear understanding of known human performance pitfalls such as fixation errors, ineffective team communication, misunderstandings, medication errors, unclear task management, and erroneous assumptions. While anesthesia professionals’ knowledge and skill are key strengths needed for safe patient care, addressing the limitations will help them to actively avoid or mitigate the risk of adverse events.

  • One approach to understanding and intervening in human performance issues for anesthesia care, especially focused on challenging situations, is that of crisis resource management (CRM). CRM (as in “cockpit [then crew] resource management”) was developed in aviation first but then adapted to health care, initially for anesthesia care, in the early 1990s. There are many formulations of CRM but they typically highlight situation awareness, dynamic decision making, task management, communication, and teamwork. The introduction of CRM in anesthesiology and its spread into many other health care disciplines and domains have typically been associated with the use of realistic simulation-based training of anesthesia professionals in single-discipline or combined team training. It also has helped focus attention on systems’ issues that relate to key aspects of human performance highlighted in CRM-oriented training.

  • “First do no harm”: Every avoidable harm or death to a patient is one tragic event too many. Anesthesia professionals must strive to avoid all harm that they may potentially impose, knowingly or not, on patients. Future progress on patient safety and human performance in anesthesia will require interdisciplinary research and training, improvements in systems thinking and systems safety, organizational learning, and the involvement of all levels of the health care industry.

Acknowledgment

The editors, publisher, and Drs. Marcus Rall, David M. Gaba, and Peter Dieckmann would like to thank Dr. Steven K. Howard for his contribution to this chapter in the prior edition of this work. It has served as the foundation for the current chapter.

What this Chapter is About: An Overview

This chapter provides the reader with an overview of key human performance and safety issues concerning anesthesiology and demonstrates the relevance of those topics to the clinical performance of anesthesia professionals. Applying the knowledge of this chapter to patient care can help to avoid unnecessary harm to patients and also prevent psychological harm to the anesthesia professional (as the “second victim”). Hence, this chapter is not only about patient safety, but also about the anesthesia professional’s safety and well-being as care provider. The authors provide a set of practical safety concepts and strategies to guide the reader in improving or refreshing case management-related skills and to sensitize the reader to safety-related core issues and competencies in anesthesia.

Because most of the work on human performance has focused on anesthesiology in the operating room (OR), this chapter deals primarily with aspects of performance and safety in that setting. Nevertheless, most of the same principles and issues are relevant to other perioperative settings, to critical care, and to a lesser degree, in pain medicine. They will also largely apply to emergency medicine and other health care domains sharing similar cognitive profiles. For readers with a special interest in intensive care, a selection of references is given as a starting point.

The chapter contains references ranging from several decades old to quite recent. The authors have tried to balance retention of classic references, where the intellectual content has only changed slightly over the years, with the introduction of current literature that reflects changes in thinking or evidence, or newer syntheses of knowledge and experience.

In this chapter, the authors use “anesthesia professional” to refer to any anesthesia clinician taking care of a patient, whether a physician, certified registered nurse anesthetist (CRNA), or anesthesia assistant (or to similar positions in other countries).

Readers Will Learn

  • … safety relevant aspects of a dynamic and complex work environment and the resulting consequences for clinicians. Several sections highlight the nature of anesthesia as a highly complex and dynamic working environment and the difficulties that arise for human performance and patient safety.

  • … characteristics and risks of different tasks performed by anesthesia professionals and countermeasures to mitigate their potential risks.

  • … the issues of human performance, human limitations, and various relevant safety strategies that address them for both individuals and teams.

  • … aspects of system safety concerning high-reliability organizations (HROs).

What this Chapter is Not About

The literature related to human performance and patient safety is vast. Standard reference works are available as well as several Internet sources ( Appendix 6.1 ). This chapter samples only a portion of this literature as it most closely relates to the work of anesthesia professionals. This chapter does not address in detail human-machine interactions and the physical design of the work environment. These aspects of human factors, or ergonomics, in anesthesiology are important in their own right. The reader is referred to several publications that review these issues in detail. Also not part of this chapter are most issues of infection control and medication safety, even as they pertain to the perioperative arena. Here, too, the reader is referred to several publications that review these issues in detail and also to other chapters in this book.

Human Performance and Patient Safety in Anesthesia: Why is this Important?

Even though provision of anesthesia has become a “safe” discipline over the last decades through many scientific and technical improvements, anesthesia per se is an intrinsically hazardous undertaking. Many aspects of anesthetic drugs and care can affect vital human functions and are potentially lethal while they are not therapeutic in themselves. Evolution did not intend for human beings to be rendered temporarily insensitive to pain, unconscious, amnestic, and in many cases paralyzed. The surgical procedure itself may cause or trigger a variety of physiologic derangements and some patients needing anesthesia are already severely ill. Thus, in anesthesiology a stable situation can turn into a life-threatening situation in seconds, minutes, or hours, whereas in many arenas of health care changes happen in days, months, or years.

Medical and technical skills alone are not sufficient for excellent medical care. Historically, an adequately trained anesthesia professional was automatically assumed to always perform appropriately. Deviations from optimal outcomes were understood to result from imperfections in the art and science of anesthesiology. This perception led to heavy emphasis on the scientific and technical aspects of anesthesia training and care. Adverse outcomes were mostly ascribed to unavoidable side effects of a medication, underlying patient disease, negligence, or incompetence on the part of the anesthesia professional.

Interestingly, research shows that the etiology of most adverse events is generally not related to intrinsic problems associated with equipment, drugs, or diseases, but rather that 80% of the avoidable events are caused by so-called human factors (HF)—similar to the statistics emanating from the aviation industry. For instance, a review of critical anesthesia incidents by Cooper and associates revealed that human factors were contributory in 82% of the 359 incidents reported. These incidents ranged from simple equipment malfunction in some cases, to death in others, indicating the seriousness and importance of the problems. Data from an earlier evaluation of 2000 incident reports support these findings with 83% of incidents occurring due to human error. The term HF describes the physical and psychological behavior of humans in relation to specific environments, jobs, organizational patterns, machines, products, and individual challenges.

The performance of human beings is incredibly flexible, powerful, and robust in some aspects but limited and vulnerable in others. Today we have a more complete understanding of the human performance of anesthesia professionals than existed decades ago. For example, we know that the successful conduct of an anesthetic depends on having the requisite technical skills and relevant pathophysiologic knowledge. But it has also become clear that the effective real-time implementation of such expertise to a large extent depends on several nonmedical and nontechnical elements of performance. Among HF issues are those that are termed human performance-shaping factors like fatigue, boredom, and distraction.

Human factor-related safety strategies for the individual and the team are indispensable. Lapses, mistakes, and errors have the potential to harm a patient (“first victim”), but can also harm professionals themselves (“second victim”). Professionals suffer as second victims largely from the perceived guilt about an error that led to actual harm. The clinical institution involved may also suffer financially or in reputation from such events, although often the occurrence of these events are not known to the public, and unlike in other industries (e.g., aviation, chemical manufacturing) there is no direct harm to the physical means of production. Nonetheless, the best way to avoid harm to the professional or the organization is to prevent adverse events or mitigate harm to patients.

Organizational safety attitudes are essential to support high individual human performance. More attention should be devoted to training anesthesia professionals in human performance issues so that they can develop and apply core competencies for achieving human performance on a daily basis. Moreover, departmental and organizational leadership must understand the enormous impact their attitudes and behavior have in shaping human performance, safety culture, outcomes, and ultimately (in all likelihood), the level of patient safety.

Even in anesthesiology, still a long way to go. Historically, anesthesiology was the first medical specialty to specifically focus on the promotion of patient safety. As a consequence, anesthesiology is widely recognized as the pioneering leader in patient safety efforts. Compared to other medical disciplines, the track record of anesthesiology is indeed a model of patient safety for the rest of health care. However, safety science teaches us that patient safety and quality improvement are never-ending processes and complacency is dangerous. In addition, the increasing “production pressure” in anesthesia practice from expanding clinical demands in the face of constant or diminishing resources may threaten previously won gains. Any patient harmed by an anesthetic is one patient too many. This approach is aligned with the zero vision statement of the U.S. Anesthesia Patient Safety Foundation (APSF): “That no one shall be harmed by anesthesia care.” In this regard Cooper and Gaba wrote that anesthesia professionals “… should remain aware of the hazards they still face, take pride in having been the leaders in patient safety efforts, and stay motivated to continue the pursuit of ‘no harm from anesthesia’ with the passion it still demands” (p. 1336).

Saving hearts, brains, and lives. Several recently published studies demonstrate the benefits of implementing various patient safety strategies. The authors have experienced the benefit of a more safety- and human performance-focused approach in their own work, as have their colleagues who also work in this field or in other fields that are endeavoring to create a safer health care system. Although it may be challenging to produce undisputable evidence that patient outcomes are improved by addressing the issues and implementing the strategies presented in this chapter, there is strong reason and ongoing research to support the belief that hearts, brains, and lives have indeed been saved by applying them. That belief is reward enough for the efforts of the authors as they share with the reader what is known about human performance and patient safety.

Nature of the Anesthesia Professional’s Operational Domain: A Dynamic and Complex Environment

The practice of anesthesiology can be characterized as a dynamic and complex environment that presents the anesthesia professional with challenges that may jeopardize human performance and patient safety.

To better understand these patient safety challenges that are related to human performance, the authors first describe the key characteristics of anesthesia work. In what follows they address (1) critical factors that categorize anesthesiology as a complex and dynamic working environment; (2) the safety challenge of inherent asymmetry between safety and production, and the effects of production pressure; and (3) the safety challenge of complexity and tight coupling in the anesthesia domain.

Anesthesiology by its Nature Involves Crises

What makes anesthesiology and a few other medical domains (such as intensive care medicine, emergency medicine, obstetrics, neonatology, and surgery, to name a few) different from most other medical fields? The answer is that the clinical environment of anesthesiology is both complex and dynamic which, when combined with the inherent risks of surgery and anesthesia, makes crisis situations frequent and challenging to deal with. These moments of terror necessitate that anesthesia professionals be expert in crisis management.

Criteria defining a complex and dynamic world Based on the work of Orasanu and colleagues, the following text describes some of the characteristics of anesthesia that make it a complex and dynamic world.

  • 1.

    Ill-structured problems. In contrast to well-structured problems, the nature and the goal of ill-structured problems are often vague or unclear, and many problem elements remain unknown or ambiguous. In anesthesiology, the patient’s physiologic behavior is not an independent random variable but is causally linked to previous decisions and actions. There often is not just a single problem with a single decision to be made, but rather a variety of interrelated problems. Interdependent decisions must be made and actions taken by the anesthesia professional, surgeon, and other perioperative personnel.

  • 2.

    Uncertain system. The patient is the main “system” of immediate interest to the anesthesia professional, just as the aircraft is of immediate interest to the pilot. Patients are intrinsically very complex, and they contain many components with underlying functions that are imperfectly understood. The medical world knows very little about the underlying causes of specific physiologic events, although the general principles involved can be described. Unlike industrial or aviation systems, patients are not designed, built, or tested by humans, nor do they come with an operator’s manual. The true state of the patient cannot usually be measured directly. It must be inferred from ambiguous patterns of clinical observations and data from electronic monitors. These data are imperfect because, unlike industrial systems that are designed and built with sensors in key areas to measure the most important variables, patients are typically instrumented to measure the variables that are easiest to monitor, predominantly with the use of noninvasive methods. Most physiologic functions are observed indirectly through weak signals available at the body surface that are prone to various types of electrical and mechanical interference. Invasive measurements are also vulnerable to artifacts and uncertainties of interpretation. Even if the anesthesia professional knew the exact state of the patient, the patient’s response to interventions would be unpredictable, as normal patients show genetic or acquired differences in reflex sensitivity, pharmacokinetics, or pharmacodynamics that can yield a wide range of responses to a given dose of a drug or to a routine action (e.g., laryngoscopy). In diseased or traumatized patients, or in the presence of acute abnormalities, these responses may be markedly abnormal, and patients may overreact or underreact to otherwise appropriate actions. Thus, the patient as a system has substantially greater uncertainty than do engineered systems.

  • 3.

    Dynamic environment. Dynamism stems from the frequency of routine and anomalous changes or events, the rapidity with which they evolve, and the unpredictability of the patient’s physiology and response to interventions. An anesthetized patient is in a constant state of change during surgery, with many events outside the anesthesia professional’s control, such as when the surgeon inadvertently transects a major vessel or when a patient with a previously unknown allergy suffers anaphylaxis. Although preventive measures can reduce the likelihood of some events, other events cannot be totally avoided because they are inevitable side effects of medically necessary procedures (e.g., surgical blood loss). Unpredictable and dynamic occurrences compete with the preplanned aspects of the case and together they drive the anesthesia professional’s actions.

  • 4.

    Time stress. Because the OR is a scarce resource, an incessant overall time pressure exists to use the OR efficiently (see section “Production Pressure”). Surgeons or OR managers pressing to start a case may affect the anesthesia professional’s decisions and actions that could jeopardize safety standards. Over the long run this can cause a systematic “normalization of deviance” (see section “Normalization of Deviance and Flirting with the Margin”), meaning the emergence of new, less stringent standard behaviors that are seen as normal judgments that previously would have been viewed as aberrant. An even more intense time stress occurs within a case when dynamic situations evolve rapidly and become time critical.

  • 5.

    Shifting, ill-defined, or competing goals. Multiple goals of case management may compete with each other. (e.g., hemodynamic stability vs. good operating conditions for the surgeon vs. rapid emergence from anesthesia). The OR manager’s administrative goals (high throughput, low cost) may sometimes compete with those of the anesthesia professional. All these goals shift as the patient’s situation changes dynamically throughout a procedure and the flow of cases changes throughout the work day. For example, decisions on surgical operation planning are heavily influenced and manipulated by micropolitics and power, as investigated by Engelmann and colleagues. Nurok and colleagues portray this aspect in their survey: “Are surgeons and anesthesiologists lying to each other or gaming the system?”

  • 6.

    Short action feedback loops. The time constants of actions and their effects are very short, on the order of seconds to minutes. Complete intermixing of decision making and action occurs; these functions are not performed in separate cycles. Most decisions and actions are implemented incrementally, constantly evaluating the relative success or failure of actions-to-date to determine how best to proceed. Anesthesia professionals often do not jump to conclusions or implement a whole set of actions all at once but try one or two approaches and see how they work, constantly reassessing rather than jumping ahead too far at once.

  • 7.

    High stakes. The decisions and actions taken by anesthesia professionals can determine the outcome for the patient. The stakes are high because even for elective surgery in healthy patients, the risk of catastrophe is ever-present. Death, brain damage, or other permanent injury may be the end result of many pathways that can begin with seemingly innocuous triggering events. Each intervention, even if appropriate, is associated with side effects, some of which are themselves serious. Some risks cannot be anticipated or avoided. Unlike an event such as a commercial flight, which can be delayed or aborted if a problem occurs or if the weather is bad, these options are not always possible in health care. Sometimes immediate surgery (and anesthesia) may be necessary to treat a medical problem that is itself life threatening. Balancing the risks of anesthesia and surgery against the risk of the patient’s underlying diseases can be extremely difficult.

  • 8.

    Multiple players. Perioperative domains involve multiple players from different professional backgrounds. Each profession has its own characteristics. On the one hand, surgeons, anesthesia professionals and OR nurses all want safety and a good outcome for the patient. On the other hand, each discipline and profession has other inherent goals. For example, surgeons may usually seem eager to perform the surgery and may seem to be more willing to take risks and to view the probability of a good outcome with optimism. In contrast, anesthesiologists may tend to be rather risk averse. Also, for a variety of reasons, it seems that surgeons tend to put production pressure on anesthesia professionals and nurses more than the other way around. The idiosyncrasies of interaction among various individual OR team members sometimes dominate the work environment. The OR sometimes offers a unique team structure of action teams, where the members may vary greatly from one day to the next (see later section “Teamwork”). Furthermore, there is a certain individual variation in the performance of each member; on any given day even usually “good” people may not be at their best.

  • 9.

    Organizational goals and norms. The anesthesia professional works within the formal and informal norms of the OR suite, the anesthesia department, the institution, and the professional culture as a whole. Sometimes anesthesia professionals feel pressured to make decisions that they do not believe are best for the patient in order to comply with these norms. Therefore, it is important to face human performance and patient safety pitfalls not only on the individual and team level, but also on a larger departmental and organizational level (see later section “Patient Safety on the Organizational Level: Issues and Strategies”).

Although some of these aforementioned characteristics apply to other domains of medicine, anesthesiology is unique in that many of the characteristics are prominent. In particular, what sets anesthesia apart from clinic-based or ward-based medicine is the intensity of the dynamics, time pressure, uncertainty, and extreme variation within the complexity, with danger lurking just below the surface, as well as the unique team constellation of so-called action teams (see section “Human Factors on the Team Level”).

Other factors influencing complexity in anesthesia: device variety and tight coupling. The complexity in anesthesia also stems from the variety of devices in use and their interconnections. The challenge is that the equipment often consists of a proliferation of independent devices with multiple, nonstandardized interconnections. Devices seem often to be designed by engineers in isolation. As a consequence, interactions between devices, or among the equipment, the patient, and the human operator, may not be adequately addressed in the design phase.

Furthermore, complexity in anesthesia derives from complex interactions which are highly interdependent (tightly coupled). Coupling describes the notion of relations between parts of a system, which either can be tightly or loosely coupled. Because many body systems affect each other, the patient is a major site of tight coupling. The anesthetic state tends to erode the protective and compensatory physiologic buffers among some of these interconnected systems, thereby forcing the patient’s system to become even more connected and strengthening the coupling between them and between the patient and external technologic supports (e.g., ventilator or infusions of hemodynamically active drugs).

Production Pressure Resulting in Asymmetry Between Safety and Production

The current trend of increasing production pressure in perioperative care can further strain the working conditions in this demanding work environment. Social and organizational environments may act as a source of production pressure on anesthesia professionals.

Safety attitudes compete against economic thinking. Production pressure encompasses the economic and social pressures placed on workers to consider production, not safety, their primary priority. In anesthesiology, this typically means starting cases early, keeping the OR schedule moving speedily, with few cancellations and minimum time between cases. In principle, safety and efficiency can go hand in hand. Many aspects of high reliability, such as standard operating procedures, preprocedure briefings, and flattening the hierarchy, may smooth operation of the system, as well as make it safer. However, the pressure for throughput as well as the wish to please the surgeon or the OR manager, or the attempt to make up time by skipping essential procedures can erode safety and lead to a normalization of deviance (the new normal, see sections “Normalization of Deviance” and & “Flirting with the Margin”). For example, when anesthesia professionals succumb to production pressures, they may skip appropriate preoperative evaluation and planning, or they may not perform adequate pre-use checkout of equipment. Even when preoperative evaluation does take place, overt or covert pressure from surgeons (or others) can cause anesthesia professionals to proceed with elective cases despite the existence of serious or uncontrolled medical problems.

Production pressure as a trigger to depart inappropriately from standard operating procedures and standards. Production pressure can cause anesthesia professionals to choose techniques that they would otherwise believe to be inadvisable. Gaba and associates reported on a survey of a large random sample of California anesthesiologists concerning their experience with production pressure. A nontrivial minority of respondents (20% to 40%) reported meaningful levels of pressure to conform to such pressures, to make decisions against their judgment of optimal safety, and to risk economic consequences if they act as they see appropriate. Generally, the pressures were already internalized after prior unpleasant experiences rather than stemming from blatant external attacks. Although there are anecdotal reports of increasing production pressure, as well as organizational practices that increase it (e.g., scheduling elective cases to start late at night or after midnight without separate shifts of anesthesia professionals), there has been no comparable repeated survey of such pressures in recent years. Fully investigating these aspects of the work environment is difficult because such relationships are driven by economic considerations, as well as by the complex organizational and interpersonal networks linking the different medical cultures. Changing the environment will be equally challenging and calls for organizational action (see section “Patient Safety Strategies on the Individual and Team Level: Crisis Resource Management [CRM] and Other Training Curricula”).

The efficiency-thoroughness trade-off (ETTO). Given the limited resources in the health care system, professionals constantly need to prioritize and make trade-offs, the most common being the efficiency-thoroughness trade-off (ETTO) described in detail by Eric Hollnagel in his book The ETTO Principle: Efficiency-Thoroughness Trade-Off: Why Things that Go Right Sometimes Go Wrong . One example of a tradeoff is reducing the amount of information sought about individual preoperative patients in order to more efficiently process large numbers of them. Since it rarely is possible to be both effective and thorough at the same time, the balance of the trade-off can get into an unnoticed disequilibrium threatening human performance and patient safety.

Inherent imbalance between signals of safety and signals of production. One of the challenges in achieving optimal safety is the asymmetry of the signals of safety and the signals of production : (1) Investments for production are easy to plan for and measure. Feedback about production is easy to obtain (revenue, earnings, expenses) and to interpret (success, no success). Success is indicated positively (more production, more earnings) and reinforcing. The relationship between the application of resources (money, effort, time) and production goals is relatively certain. (2) On the contrary, investments for safety are more difficult to plan for and the costs and benefits can only be measured indirectly and without continuity, making them difficult to interpret or even deceptive. Feedback about safety is inherently weak and ambiguous. Success is less reinforcing because if indicated negatively (fewer accidents or incidents)—how can one measure the accidents that could have occurred but did not? The relationship between the application of resources (money, effort, time) and safety goals is equally uncertain. There have been many occasions when only after a catastrophe takes place are the signals concerning a safety hazard understood, and often there is evidence that some personnel did recognize the hazards but either did not sufficiently press the issue or else they were systematically ignored or repressed.

Nature of the Anesthesia Professional’s Work: Task Variation and Workload Management

As previously described, the operational domain of anesthesia can be considered as a complex and dynamic world, managing different challenges with uncertain systems, competing goals, time pressure, and multiple players with special team constellations. All of these can affect human performance and patient safety. There also exist the challenges of the different specific tasks of conducting an anesthetic, whether manual (i.e., insertion of cannula, intubation), behavioral (i.e., leadership behaviors, communication patterns), or cognitive (i.e., attention, preparedness, dynamic decision making). Safe and efficient performance requires both medical and non-medical skills.

Human failure or equipment failure can have disastrous consequences. Errors in cognitive tasks as well as cognitive biases are common in anesthesiology and pose a threat to patient safety. In the following section the authors provide insights into the nature of the anesthesiaology professional’s many tasks and their various vulnerabilities. This is important not only for individual and team improvements but also for improvements concerning clinical education, training, organizational structures, and equipment design. The focus of the upcoming section is on manual and cognitive tasks. The behavioral non-medical are discussed later (see section “Patient Safety on the Individal and Team Level”).

This section briefly highlights the different phases of an anesthetic regimen and summarizes findings of task analysis and task performance studies, provides facts about the anesthesia machine checkout protocol as a safety relevant task, and introduces nonobservable cognitive tasks of administering anesthesia, in particular dynamic decision making. In addition, this section provides an introduction to workload measurement methodologies, gives study results concerning the performance of anesthesia professionals summarizes the benefits and obstacles of human performance measures, and highlights results from task analysis studies.

Procedural Tasks of Anesthesia Professionals and Related Vulnerabilities

Multiple task analysis studies have investigated what actions and thought processes an anesthesiologist is required to perform to achieve good anesthetic care. Since the 1970s, numerous studies have been done either by direct observation during real cases or by indirect observation during cases captured on videotape. In addition, an increasing number of studies have been performed in realistic simulation environments. Of note, many of the studies cited are pioneering ones that remain valid today.

The early studies of the work of anesthesiologists drew attention to the wide spectrum of tasks in the trajectory of perioperative care. They highlighted that many tasks must be done in close parallel with others (approximating multitasking, see section Task Management), showing not only the different tasks and their substeps that can be prone to error, but also different phases of task intensity during an anesthetic. Subsequent task analysis studies focused on the workload and the performance of the anesthesiologist, at a later time expanding to performance measures based on teamwork, communication, and leadership. More recently, task analysis studies have been performed with respect to ergonomic equipment design questions. Certain very complex issues concerning human-machine interactions and the ways in which technology affects behavior in complex patient-care environments are beyond the scope of this chapter; however a number of publications address these issues.

The different phases for an anesthetic regimen are commonly classified into (1) preoperative planning, (2) induction, (3) maintenance of, and (4) emergence from anesthesia. Every phase is characterized by manual and cognitive tasks, each of which consists of further subordinated steps and each of which presents with a variable density of tasks and human error pitfalls. For a comprehensive review, the reader is directed to the publication of Phipps and colleagues for detailed information.

Preoperative Planning

The anesthesia professional needs to be prepared for active intervention during the whole anesthetic regimen. Part of this preparedness involves obtaining the necessary equipment and supplies, preparing medications, and conducting pre-use checkouts of life-support equipment and the anesthesia machine before induction (See section “Pre-use Checkout of Equipment/Anesthesia Machine Checkout”).

However, with 44 task steps as identified in the task analysis study by Phipps and associates, the equipment check is a lengthy and detailed process, and it is possible that steps may be omitted, either intentionally or unintentionally.

Induction

Task analysis studies demonstrated increased anesthesiologist workload during induction, emergence, and emergency surgery. Phipps and associates identified 73 task steps between the preparation of drugs and transferring the anesthetized patient to the operating room (induction phase), including cognitive and communicative tasks, machine checkouts, as well as a considerable number of manual task steps, such as the insertion of the cannula and airway devices. Pape and Dingman examined the number of unrelated distractions during the induction process (i.e., unrelated questions of other personnel, OR doors opening and closing, noise, answering incoming telephone calls, unrelated communication), discovering an average of 7.5 total interruptions per 9 minutes. They argued that interruptions and distractions can lead to loss of focus and result in errors, and requested further research to determine whether silence during induction is needed as a safety measure. Another study concluded that on average during cases one distractive event occurs every 4 minutes 23 seconds, with approximately 3.4 distractions during induction and 3.0 distractions when moving from the induction room to the OR. In this study while most distracting events had no negative consequences for the patient, 22% had negative consequences (suboptimal management). Interestingly 3% were actually not distractions because they had positive consequences for the patient. In another study, 20% of visual attention during induction was directed to the patient monitor, increasing up to 30% during simulated critical incident induction scenarios. During the observation of real cases, yet another study group found that drug/fluid tasks comprised 20 ± 6% of induction, 15 ± 8% of maintenance, and 12 ± 7% of emergence during routine cases.

Maintenance Phase of Anesthesia

Betza and colleagues found in an observational study that anesthesia providers spent 71% of their time during maintenance doing patient or display monitoring tasks. Transitions between the task categories occurred approximately once every 9 seconds. It appeared that regardless of the task, there was a high frequency of task transitions to look at the visual displays and then from the visual displays toward the patient. Compared with the induction phase, there are fewer (16) task steps during the maintenance phase. However, there is evidence that a relatively high proportion of critical incidents occur during the maintenance phase (59% of incidents during maintenance, 26% during induction). Patients’ conditions may vary in an overt or subtle way. Therefore, anesthesia professionals need to continuously monitor several parameters. Their attention may be distracted or misbalanced, as not all parameters need the same level of attention all the time. Sometimes other tasks, such as telephone calls, auscultation, insertion of an arterial cannula, use of transesophageal echocardiography (TEE), and “problem solving,” may divert the attention of the anesthesia professional. As attention is a limited resource and susceptible to distractions, it is important to learn how to best allocate one’s attention in continuously changing and complex environments like anesthesia (see later section, “Situation Awareness”).

Very detailed task analyses took place in a series of studies carried out by the University of California, San Diego (UCSD), Stanford University, the San Diego and Stanford Veterans Administration Medical Centers. Generally, studies of workload indicate that induction and, to a lesser degree, emergence are the most intensive. However, it is also argued that many of those tasks performed are part of a routine, which tends to reduce the effort required. Maintenance, in contrast, is typically less physically “action dense” but mental activity continues as a wide range of information is used and processed.

Emergence

With 40 task steps to carry out in a relatively short period of time, the discontinuation of anesthesia and subsequent transfer of the patient to recovery is fairly busy. A study from Broom and associates suggests that emergence is the most distractive period compared with induction and maintenance, finding noise during emergence at 58 decibels (dB) (compared to induction at 46 dB and maintenance at 52 dB), with sudden loud noise (>70 dB) occurring more frequently during emergence than at induction or maintenance. The range of staff entrances and exits were also highest during emergence (10), compared to induction (0) and maintenance (6). Conversations unrelated to the procedure occurred in 93% of emergences. Emergence also was found to be the period of most frequent distractions, occurring on average every 2 minutes. Those findings are acknowledged by the following quotations retrieved from subjective study interviews:

“I don’t think people quite appreciate that emergence is as important as induction really and sometimes they’re just glad to have finished off their case. They’re crashing and banging and moving on and fail to realize.” and “(…) [B]asically as far as they’re concerned the job’s finished. They’re there moaning, yelling, or talking about the next case. I do find that distracting because I think: ‘we haven’t actually finished this case yet’.” (p. 711)

Patient Safety Action Box

The period of the patient’s emergence from anesthesia is high risk and a high workload for the anesthesia professional while low stress for other OR personnel. Often they forget and the noise level may rise considerably. If so the anesthesia professional should politely demand thoughtfulness and quiet from the other OR team members. This would also be a possible setting to invoke the auditory “sterile cockpit” protocol (see later section “Distractions and Interruptions in the Operating Room”).

Pre-Use Checkout of Equipment/Anesthesia Machine Checkout

A pre-use check to ensure the correct functioning of anesthetic equipment is essential to patient safety. Failure to check anesthesia equipment prior to use can lead to patient injury or “near misses.” Based on a retrospective incident analysis of 668 reported incidents, Marcus reported a total of nearly 18% of in-theater incidents in pediatric anesthesia resulted from the failure to check. More recent generations of anesthesia machines have internal computers that can conduct checks of many aspects of machine functioning and alert the anesthesia professional to problems. However, the authors have observed in simulations of embedded machine problems or external equipment faults (e.g., nitrous oxide vs. oxygen swap) that anesthesia professionals may lack a complete understanding of these systems.

In the United States an updated machine checklist was released by the American Society of Anesthesiologists (ASA) in 2008. Because no specific checkout recommendation could be applicable to all modern anesthesia delivery systems and to all anesthetizing locations, the latest recommendation is based on a set of design guidelines for the pre-anesthetic checkout and provides samples of checkout procedures (available at: www.asahq.org/resources/clinical-information/2008-asa-recommendations-for-pre-anesthesia-checkout ). The 2008 pre-use anesthesia apparatus checkout recommendation (AACR) contains a list of 15 separate items that should be checked at the beginning of each day (preoperative check) or whenever a machine is moved, serviced, or the vaporizers changed. Eight of these items should be checked prior to each procedure (preinduction check). Some of the steps may be already part of an automated manufacturer’s checkout process in the anesthesia machine; others need to be performed individually. Feldman and associates state:

“Following these checklists will typically require <5 minutes at the beginning of the day, and <2 minutes between cases, but will provide you with the confidence that the machine will be able to provide all essential life support functions before you begin a case.” (p. 6)

In 2012, the Association of Anaesthetists of Great Britain and Ireland (AAGBI) released a new safety guideline on checking anesthesia equipment that also includes, but is not limited to, the pre-use checkout of the anesthetic machine (available at: https://www.aagbi.org/sites/default/files/checking_anaesthetic_equipment_2012.pdf ). Rather recently revised guidelines on pre-use checking of the anesthetic equipment including the anesthesia workstation have been published by the Australian and New Zealand College of Anaesthetists (ANZCA) in 2014 and by the Canadian Anesthesiologists’ Society (CAS) in 2016.

Patient Safety Action Box

There are numerous checkout checklists available. A serious patient safety issue is the non-adherence to standard protocols. The anesthetic checkout of equipment is a method of systematically ensuring the anesthetic professional executes a thorough check of the anesthetic equipment. All professionals should therefore use it as a standard practice for checking the anesthetic machine to provide the best and safest patient care. The implementation of a checklist is an organizational process that needs systematic implementation and, optimally, user training.

Cognitive Tasks of Administering Anesthesia and Related Vulnerabilities

The observable tasks do not tell the whole story of what the anesthesia professional is doing. Even when the anesthesia professional appears idle, most of the time mental activity is ongoing. Several investigators have written about the cognitive elements in anesthesiology. Of those errors made, cognitive errors and cognitive biases in anesthesiology are common and pose a threat to patient safety. In the following section, (1) the cognitive tasks of dynamic decision making and situation awareness are described and summarized in the anesthesia professional’s core cognitive process model; (2) subsequently, the management and coordination of the core cognitive process model are discussed in this section; and (3) several methodologies to measure cognitive workload are touched on.

Introduction of the Anesthesia Professional’s Core Cognitive Process Model

Besides the constant check whether anticipated milestones of the anesthetic regimen are achieved and the constant check of incoming data streams, the anesthesia professional must also react to a large number of contingencies, some of which can be predicted in advance based on the patient’s history and the type of surgery, whereas others cannot. If so, the existing plan may have to be reactively modified.

Different aspects of decision making and situation awareness are summarized in the anesthesia professional’s core cognitive process model. The model was developed by David Gaba and draws heavily on the work of a number of other investigators who studied human performance in a variety of complex, dynamic worlds. It is described in detail as a framework for understanding the empiric data, and provides a vocabulary for discussing the elements of both successful and unsuccessful performance by anesthesia professionals.

The entire core process model, shown in Fig. 6.1 , depicts the anesthesia professional as working at five different interacting cognitive levels (resource management level, procedural level, communication level, abstract reasoning level, supervisory control level) to implement and control a core process of observation, verification, problem recognition, prediction of future states, decision making, action, and reevaluation ( Box 6.1 ). The core process must then be integrated with the behavior of other team members and with the constraints of the work environment. Expert performance in anesthesia involves these features in a repeated loop of the different steps. Errors can occur at each step in this process.

Fig. 6.1, Core cognitive process model of the anesthetist’s complex real-time problem-solving behavior (see text for detailed description).

Box 6.1
Elements of the Core Cognitive Process of an Anesthesiologist

  • 1.

    Observation

  • 2.

    Verification

  • 3.

    Problem recognition

  • 4.

    Prediction of future states

  • 5.

    Decision making

    • a.

      Application of precompiled responses (recognition-primed decision making)

    • b.

      Decision making using heuristics and probability

    • c.

      Decision making including abstract reasoning

  • 6.

    Action implementation

  • 7.

    Reevaluation (avoiding fixation errors)

  • 8.

    Start again with 1 (loop continues)

The division of mental activities into levels follows the work of Rasmussen and Reason et al.. Having multiple levels supports the concepts of parallel processing (performing more than one task at a time but working on different levels of mental activity) and multitasking/multiplexing (performing only one task at a time but switching very rapidly from one task to another), as shown in several task analysis studies. Table 6.1 gives an overview and a brief explanation of the different mental activity levels.

Table 6.1
Levels of Mental Activity
Level of Control Explanation Comments
Resource management level Command and control of all resources, including teamwork and communication Incident analysis shows a huge contribution of lack of resource management and communication skills to the development of incidents and accidents; the importance of these factors is reflected in the ACRM principles and simulation training courses (see Chapter 7 )
Supervisory control level Metacognition: thinking about thinking Dynamic adaptation of the thought process, decision making (e.g., avoiding fixation errors), scheduling, and remembering actions (e.g., prospective memory tasks)
Abstract reasoning level Use of fundamental medical knowledge, search for high-level analogies, deductive reasoning Often in parallel with other levels; in emergency situations often too slow and too sensitive to distractions in high-workload situations
Procedural level Precompiled responses, following algorithms, heuristics, “reflexes” Recognition-primed decision making—experts are more often on this level; special errors may occur as a result of not checking for the appropriateness of the “procedure”; less experienced personnel may misuse this level for ill-considered, unadapted “cookbook medicine”
Sensorimotor level Use of all senses and manual actions; “feeling, doing, hearing”; sometimes subconscious control of actions Experts perform smooth action sequences and control their actions by direct feedback from their senses (e.g., action sequences of placing an intravenous line or endotracheal intubation; skill-based errors such as slips and lapses may occur)
ACRM, Anesthesia crisis resource management.

At the sensorimotor level, activities involving sensory perception or motor actions take place with minimal conscious control; they are smooth, practiced, and highly integrated patterns of behavior. At the procedural level, the anesthesia professional performs regular routines in a familiar work situation. These routines have been derived and internalized from training and from previous work episodes. A level of abstract reasoning is used during preoperative planning, and intraoperatively it is used in unfamiliar situations for which no well-practiced expertise or routine is available from previous encounters. Rasmussen’s model was extended by the explicit addition of two additional levels of mental activity—the supervisory and the resource management level—that provide for dynamic adaptation of the anesthesia professional’s own thought processes. Supervisory control is concerned with dynamically allocating finite attention between routine and non-routine actions, among multiple problems or themes, and among the five cognitive levels. Attention is such a scarce resource, therefore its allocation is extremely important in every aspect of dynamic decision making. Resource management deals with the command and control of available resources, including teamwork and communication. Expert performance in anesthesia involves these features in a repeated loop. An overview of the core cognitive process and its elements is given in Box 6.1 . The elements are explained in detail in the following text sections and include (1) observation; (2) verification; (3) problem recognition; (4) prediction of future states; (5) precompiled responses; (6) action taking/action implementation; and (7) reevaluation.

Observation

Anesthesia professionals use observations to decide whether the patient’s course is on track or whether a problem is occurring; this is the first step of the decision making cycle. Data are observed and transformed by interpretation into information, followed by further interpretation into meaning. Data streams typically involve direct visual, auditory, or tactile contact with the patient, the surgical field, routine electronic monitoring, special (sometimes invasive) monitoring systems, contents of suction canisters and sponges, reading of reports of laboratory test results, and communications from other personnel. Loeb showed that anesthesiologists typically observe monitors for approximately 1 to 2 seconds every 10 to 20 seconds and that it usually took several observing cycles before they detected a subtle cue on the monitor. Management of rapidly changing situations requires the anesthesia professional to assess a wide variety of information sources. Because the human mind can attend closely to only one or two items at a time, the anesthesia professional’s supervisory control level must decide what information to attend to and how frequently to observe it (as later shown at CRM key point 14 “Allocate attention wisely”). Constant observation and interpretation of the different information systems is executed repeatedly throughout the course of an anesthetic regimen. The plethora of simultaneous data streams in even the most routine cases is a challenge. Vigilance, defined as the capacity to sustain attention, plays a crucial role in the observation and detection of problems and is a necessary prerequisite for meaningful care. Vigilance can be degraded by performance-shaping factors (see later section “Performance Shaping Factors”) and it can be overwhelmed by the sheer amount of information and the rapidity with which it is changing.

Verification

In the working environment of an anesthesia professional, the available, observed information is not always reliable. Most monitoring is noninvasive and indirect and is susceptible to artifacts (false data). Even direct clinical observations such as vision or auscultation can be ambiguous. Brief transients (true data of short duration) can occur that quickly correct themselves. To prevent them from skewing the decision making process and triggering precipitous actions that may have significant side effects, critical observations must be verified before the clinician can act on them. This requires the use of all available data and information and cross-checking different related data streams rather than depending solely on any single datum without sensible interpretation (as later shown in CRM key point 8 “Use all available information” and CRM key point 10 “Cross check and double check; never assume anything”). Verification uses a variety of methods, shown in Table 6.2 .

Patient Safety Action Box

Try to be sensitive to changes and do not just explain them away as normal without double-checking or using other information to determine if everything really is okay. Assume there is a big problem unless you can prove otherwise. If in doubt, it should always be assumed that the patient is at risk and that the parameter in question is real (rule out the worst case). The burden of proof is on you. Beware of too easily assuming that it is just a technical artifact.

Table 6.2
Methods for Verification of Critical Observations
Method Explanation and Example
Repeating The observation or measurement is repeated to rule out a temporary wrong value (e.g., motion artifacts during noninvasive blood pressure measurement)
Checking trend information The short-term trend is observed for plausibility of the actual value. Trends of physiologic parameters almost always follow curves, not steps
Observing a redundant channel An existing redundant channel is checked (e.g., invasive arterial pressure and cuff pressure are redundant, or heart rate from an ECG and pulse oximeter)
Correlating Multiple related (but not redundant) variables are correlated to determine the plausibility of the parameter in question (e.g., if the ECG monitor shows a flat line and “asystole” but the invasive blood pressure curve shows waves)
Activating a new monitoring device A new monitoring modality is installed (e.g., placing a pulmonary artery catheter). This also adds another parameter for the method of “correlating”
Recalibrating an instrument or testing its function The quality and reliability of a measurement are checked, and its function is tested (e.g., if the CO 2 detector shows no values, the anesthetist can exhale through it to see whether the device works). Observation of redundant channels can also help verify a value (see above)
Replacing an instrument If doubt exists about the function of a device, an entirely new instrument or an alternative backup device may be installed
Asking for help If the decision on the values remains unclear, help should be sought early to obtain a second opinion from other trained personnel
ECG, Electrocardiogram.

Problem Recognition

Having recognized a problem, how does the expert anesthesia professional respond? The classical paradigm of decision making involves a careful comparison of the evidence with various causal hypotheses that could explain the problem. This is followed by a careful analysis of all possible actions and solutions. This approach, although powerful, is relatively slow and does not work well with ambiguous or scanty evidence. Many perioperative problems faced by anesthesia professionals require quick action under uncertainty to prevent a rapid cascade to a catastrophic adverse outcome, and solution of these problems through formal deductive reasoning from first principles is just too slow. The process of problem recognition is a central feature of several theories of cognition in complex, dynamic worlds. Problem recognition involves matching sets of environmental cues to patterns that are known to represent specific types of problems. Given the high uncertainty seen in anesthesia, the available information sources cannot always disclose the existence of a problem, and even if they do, they may not specify its identity or origin. Anesthesia professionals use approximation strategies to handle these ambiguous situations; psychologists term such strategies heuristics . Stiegler and Tung give a detailed review of heuristics and other biases that affect problem recognition. One heuristic is to categorize what is happening as one of several generic problems, each of which encompasses many different underlying conditions (similarity/pattern matching). Another is to gamble on a single diagnosis (frequency gambling ) by initially choosing the single most frequent candidate event. During preoperative planning, the anesthesia professional may adjust a mental “index of suspicion” for recognizing certain specific problems anticipated for that particular patient or surgical procedure. The anesthesia professional must also decide whether a single underlying diagnosis explains all the data or whether they could come from multiple causes. This decision is important because excessive attempts to refine the diagnosis can be very costly in terms of allocation of attention. By contrast, a premature diagnosis can lead to inadequate or erroneous treatment. The use of heuristics is typical of expert anesthesia professionals and often results in considerable time savings in dealing with problems. However, it is a double-edged sword. Both frequency gambling and inappropriate allocation of attention solely to expected problems can seriously undermine problem solving when these gambles do not pay off or are not corrected in the reevaluation process.

Many of the issues that are related to problem recognition and cognition in general are discussed in more detail in the section on decision making further below, especially when dealing with the models of System I thinking and System II thinking and of recognition-primed decision making.

Prediction of Future States

Problems must be assessed in terms of their significance for the future states of the patient. Predicting future states based on the occurrence of seemingly trivial problems is a major part of the anticipatory behaviors that characterize expert crisis managers. Problems that are already critical or that can be predicted to evolve into critical incidents receive the highest priority (as later shown in CRM key point 15 “Set priorities dynamically”). Prediction of future states also influences action planning by defining the timeframe available for required actions. Cook and colleagues described “going sour” incidents in which the future state of the patient was not adequately taken into account when early manifestations of problems were apparent. One of the challenges known from research in psychology is that the human mind is not very well suited to predict future states, when things are changing in a nonlinear fashion. Under such circumstances, which are common for natural systems such as the human body, the rate of change is almost invariably underestimated, and people are surprised at the outcome.

Patient Safety Action Box

Slow but steady and sustained blood loss in a child during surgery might result in few or subtle changes in hemodynamics for some time until rapid decompensation occurs. If the weak signs of the developing problem were not detected or misjudged the ensuing catastrophe may seem to have occurred suddenly. The use of a visible trend monitoring of heart rate or blood pressure over a longer period of time can help the anesthesia professional to be better aware of changes that are not readily apparent if only the last few measurements are compared.

Precompiled Responses

Once a critical event has been observed and verified the anesthesia professional needs to respond. In complex, dynamic domains, the initial responses of experts to the majority of events stem from precompiled rules or response plans for dealing with a recognized event. This method is referred to as recognition-primed decision making, because once the event is identified, the response is well known (see later section “Decision Making”). In the anesthesia domain, these responses are usually acquired through personal experience alone, although there is a growing realization that critical response protocols must be codified explicitly and taught systematically. Experienced anesthesia professionals have been observed to rearrange, recompile, and rehearse these responses mentally based on the patient’s condition, the surgical procedure, and the problems to be expected. Ideally, precompiled responses to common problems are retrieved appropriately and executed rapidly. When the exact nature of the problem is not apparent, a set of generic responses appropriate to the overall situation may be invoked. For example, if a problem with ventilation is detected, the anesthesia professional may switch to manual ventilation at a higher fraction of inspired oxygen (FiO 2 ) while considering further diagnostic actions. However, experiments involving simulation have demonstrated that even experienced anesthesia professionals show great variability in their use of response procedures to critical situations. This finding led these investigators to target simulator-based training in the systematic training of responses to critical events.

Even the ideal use of precompiled responses is destined to fail when the problem does not have the suspected cause or when it does not respond to the usual actions. Anesthesia cannot be administered purely by precompiled “cookbook” procedures. Abstract reasoning about the problem through the use of fundamental medical knowledge still takes place in parallel with precompiled responses, even when quick action must be taken. This seems to involve a search for high-level analogies or true deductive reasoning using deep medical and technical knowledge and a thorough analysis of all possible solutions. Anesthesia professionals managing simulated crises have linked their precompiled actions to abstract medical concepts.

Taking Action/Action Implementation

Anesthesiologists need to share their attention among different cognitive levels, among tasks, and often among problems. The intensive demands on the anesthesia professional’s attention could easily swamp the available mental resources. Therefore, the anesthesia professional must strike a balance between acting quickly on every small perturbation (which requires a lot of attention) and adopting a more conservative “wait-and-see” attitude. This balance must be constantly shifted between these extremes as the situation changes. However, during simulated crisis situations, some practitioners showed great reluctance to switch from business as usual to emergency mode even when serious problems were detected. Erring too far in the direction of wait and see is an error that can be particularly catastrophic. Preparedness for active intervention in case of dynamically changing events is a key element of an anesthesiologist’s work. But how frequent is this requirement? According to the review of Wacker and Staender, adverse events in the perioperative period continue to be frequent, occur in about 30% of hospital admissions, and may be preventable in more than 50%.

Patient Safety Action Box

Once you are sure there is a big problem, it is important for the entire team to transition into emergency mode efficiently. One way of doing so is to declare the emergency out loud with appropriate force, such as: “Ok, everybody, there’s a very serious problem with the patient—probably anaphylaxis—this is a major emergency.”

At any time during an anesthetic regimen there may be multiple things to do, each of which is intrinsically appropriate, yet they cannot all be done at once. Simulation experiments have shown that anesthesia professionals sometimes have difficulty selecting, planning, and scheduling actions optimally.

Patient Safety Action Box

It helps if team members know the actions planned and the schedule of actions preferred (referred to as shared mental models). If the anesthesia professional does not provide the information, team members should check with the anesthesiologist. It also is helpful to distribute actions with clear communicated priorities and/or timeframes (CRM principle 7: “Effective Communication”, e.g., “[Name of receiver of message], prepare xxx first, then I need xxx, and after that bring me xxx,” or “After you have done xxx, let’s start xxx together”, or “In 30 minutes, please check the blood gases/blood sugar level/ etc. again…” )

A particular hallmark of anesthesiology is that the decision maker does not just decide what action is required but is often involved directly in the implementation of actions. Executing these actions requires substantial attention and may in fact impair the anesthesia professional’s mental and physical ability to perform other activities (e.g., when an action requires a sterile procedure). This is particularly an issue when other tasks have been interrupted or temporarily suspended. Prospective memory, one’s ability to remember in the future to perform an action (i.e., to complete a task) can be easily disrupted. In addition, anesthesia professionals engaged in a manual procedure are strongly constrained from performing other manual tasks or from maintaining awareness of incoming information.

Reevaluation

In order to successfully solve dynamic problems and to cope with the rapid changes and profound diagnostic and therapeutic uncertainties seen during anesthesia, the core process must include repetitive reevaluation of the situation. Thus, the reevaluation step, initiated by the supervisory control level, returns the anesthesia professional to the observation phase, but with specific assessments in mind (see also CRM key point 12, “Reevaluate repeatedly”). Only by frequently reassessing the situation can the anesthesia professional adapt to dynamic processes, since the initial diagnosis and situation assessment can be incorrect. Even actions that are appropriate to the problem are not always successful.

The process of continually updating the assessment of the situation and monitoring the efficacy of chosen actions is termed situation awareness . Situation awareness is a very interesting and important topic in analyzing performance and reasons for errors and is discussed in detail in a later section. Box 6.2 gives examples of reevaluation questions in order to maintain situation awareness.

Box 6.2
Reevaluation Questions—Maintaining Situation Awareness

  • Did the actions have any effect (e.g., did the drug reach the patient?)?

  • Is the problem getting better, or is it getting worse?

  • Are there any side effects resulting from previous actions?

  • Are there any other problems or new problems that were missed before?

  • Was the initial situation assessment/diagnosis correct?

  • What further developments can be expected in the (near) future?

Faulty reevaluation, inadequate adaptation of the plan, or loss of situation awareness can result in a type of human error termed fixation error . Fixation errors have been described in responses of professionals to abnormal situations. Avoiding and recognizing fixation errors in the field of anesthesia is covered more in detail in section “Introduction of the 15 Crisis Resource Management Key Principles.”

Management and Coordination of the Core Cognitive Process: Supervisory Control and Resource Management

Anesthesia professionals’ abilities to adapt their own thinking (metacognition—thinking about thinking) through supervisory control and resource management are key components of dynamic decision making and therefore of crisis management.

Supervisory Control

The supervisory control allocates the scarce resource of attention during multitasking, and oversees and modulates the core process. For example, determining the frequency of observation of different data streams, prioritizing diagnostic and therapeutic alternatives, actively managing workload, prioritizing and scheduling actions. Supervisory control actively manages the workload by (1) avoiding high-workload situations by anticipation and planning, (2) distributing workload over time or (3) over personnel, (4) changing the nature of the task to reduce work, or (5) minimizing distraction. More details regarding the active management of workload are touched on in the later section on CRM key point 5 “Distribute the Workload.”

Resource Management

The highest layer of metacognition and control is known as resource management—the ability to command and control all the resources at hand to care for the patient and to respond to problems. This involves translating the knowledge of what needs to be done into effective team activity by taking into account the limitations of the complex and often ill-structured perioperative domain. Resources include personnel, equipment, and supplies, both in the immediate vicinity and, when necessary, throughout the various levels of the organization. Resource management explicitly demands teamwork and crew coordination. It is not enough for the anesthesia professional to know what to do or even to be able to do each task alone. Only so much can be accomplished in a given time, and some tasks can be performed only by other skilled personnel (e.g., catheterization lab). A key responsibility of the anesthesia professional is to mobilize needed resources and to distribute the relevant goals and tasks among those available. The details of this critical function are described in the section on “Crisis Resource Management.”

Anesthesia Professional’s Workload and Methodologies to Measure It

Most anesthesia professionals have significant other responsibilities in addition to the manual, cognitive, and behavioral duties described above, for example in administration, supervision, or teaching. Depending on the task, the task density, the individual’s experience and skill, the patient’s state, and the given circumstances (production pressure, staff availability, noise, light, space, team, etc.), the workload of the anesthesia professional can change at any time during an anesthetic regimen. Cognitive resources are diminished when the workload is heavy (i.e., during task dense phases like induction, emergence, or during an emergency), leading to lower levels of performance, increased response time, decreased vigilance, and greater risk of errors.

The concept of workload is tricky to define. Hart and Staveland describe it as follows: “Workload is not an inherent property, but rather emerges from the interaction between the requirements of a task, the circumstances under which it is performed, and the skills, behaviors, and perceptions of the operator” (p. 140). Psychology literature suggests that emotions during highly demanding activities impair cognitive processing efficiency. Task analysis studies as described earlier and task (action) density studies (see later) give insights into several aspects of workload, especially facilitating workload measurements by identifying the individual work components or subtasks to be measured. However, those kind of studies do not necessarily give insight into the performance shaping aspects of workload.

Methods to measure workload include task performance via observation, subjective assessments, and physiological measures. They are described below. For a more comprehensive review, see, for example, Leedal and Smith, and Byrne.

Primary Task Performance

The primary task performance measure assesses the subject’s performance on standard work tasks (e.g., cases seen, knots tied, etc.) as they are made progressively more difficult by increasing the number of tasks, task density, or task complexity. At first, the subject is able to keep up with the increasing task load, but at some point, the workload exceeds the ability to manage it, and performance on the standard tasks decreases.

Secondary Task Probing

Secondary task probing tests the subject with a minimally intrusive secondary task that is added to the primary work tasks. The secondary task is a simple one for which performance can be objectively measured. Reaction time, finger tapping, mental arithmetic and a vibrotactile device have for example been used for this technique as a secondary task. The anesthesia professional is instructed that the primary tasks of patient care take absolute precedence over the secondary task. Therefore, assuming that the secondary task requires some of the same mental resources as the primary task, the performance of the anesthesiologist on the secondary task is an indirect reflection of the spare capacity available to deal with it: the greater the spare capacity, the lower the primary workload. Depending on the secondary task response channels (manual, voice, gesture, multiple ways) there can exist channel interference. Controversy exists about whether these probes measure “vigilance” or “workload,” although the same techniques probably measure both aspects of performance. When probes occur infrequently, are subtle, have multiple response channels, and are performed with a low level of existing workload, they are more likely to measure vigilance; when they are frequent, readily detectable, require a manual response, and are performed during a high-workload period, they probably are more indicative of spare capacity and workload.

Subjective Measures

In subjective measures, individuals are asked, most commonly in retrospect but sometimes in real time, how much load they were or are under during actual work situations. A common and validated form to assess subjective workload is the NASA TLX form. Subjective measures usually complement objective measurements of external observations, since an anesthesia professional may subjectively underestimate the workload in settings in which objective measurements demonstrate a marked reduction in spare capacity.

Physiologic Measures

The final set of techniques for assessing workload consists of physiologic measures. Visual or auditory evoked potentials have been used successfully to assess mental workload, but this technique can be used only in a static laboratory environment. Heart rate (especially certain aspects of heart rate variability) and blood pressure are other physiologic measures that have been used, but there are challenges in reliable interpretation.

Assessing the Performance of Anesthesia Professionals

Over the years several study designs investigating manual (i.e., technical aspects of a procedure), cognitive (i.e., dynamic decision making, situation awareness, vigilance) and behavioral/nontechnical (i.e., communication, teamwork, leadership) tasks were used to assess the performance of anesthesiologists. In this section, studies with special focus on anesthesia professional’s performance about action density, work experience, teaching/delegation/supervision activity, and critical incidents/emergency treatment are presented. Most of the studies were performed in a simulated medical environment.

Because much of the more recent literature recapitulates findings of the pioneering studies, in this chapter those initial studies are used but the reader also is referred to newer, selected studies, without providing an exhaustive list. The latest, large-scale study concerning the performance of anesthesiologists was published by Weinger and co-workers in 2017, examining the performance of board-certified anesthesiologists during four emergency cases. The study is also presented in detail below.

A broader view on general human performance aspects related to anesthesia can be found in the next sections on “human performance, human factors and nontechnical skills” and “system thinking.”

Performance as a function of task density

It is generally accepted that there are limits on human ability to process information, and that information overload can lead to poor performance. For example, many people will have experienced the difficulties of simultaneously trying to drive, navigate, read road signs, and listen to passengers. However, the work domain of anesthesia oftentimes seems to require exactly this kind of task density ( Fig. 6.2 ). An interdisciplinary research group performed several task analysis studies, which allowed the analysis of multiple parallel and overlapping actions (action/task density). Figs. 6.3 and 6.4 show examples of observations of 24 real OR studies. The observation data contain many short-term fluctuations (dots) ; the moving average of action density of the previous 5 minutes was charted as well (line) . Fig. 6.3 shows a complete anesthesia procedure with increases in action density during induction and emergence from anesthesia. Fig. 6.4 shows two final phases of cardiac cases involving cardiopulmonary bypass. The described task analysis technique was also used to study action sequences in simulated cases and compare them with findings in the real OR to demonstrate and evaluate the ecologic validity of simulators (see Chapter 7 ). Findings indicated that as the density of tasks per unit time increased, the dwell time on each task decreased, and vice versa. This finding has important implications for how anesthesia professionals allocate their attention.

Fig. 6.2, Illustration of how mental workload for the individual and the team may vary during anesthesia.

Fig. 6.3, Action density diagram illustrating the derived parameter “action density” from induction of anesthesia to emergence in a real anesthetic case.

Fig. 6.4, Action density during separation from cardiopulmonary bypass without and with complications.

Another interesting aspect in this respect is the hypothesis that the mental workload of novices may be lower than that of more experienced staff because they have yet to appreciate the difficulties facing them; this is termed unconscious incompetence.

Xiao and colleagues used simulations to investigate the dimensions of task complexity and their impact on crisis activities and team processes in the trauma room. They identified four components of complexity that affected team coordination in different ways. Multiple concurrent tasks led to goal conflict, task interference, and competition for access to the patient. Uncertainty regarding the case led to differences in opinion when interpreting information and difficulties when trying to anticipate the actions of other team members. The use of contingency plans caused difficulty in knowing when to switch tasks and how then to reallocate activities. Finally, a high workload caused procedures to be compressed and this deviation from normal work further increased the complexity of the situation. They suggested training in explicit communication to meet the challenges of task complexity.

Performance as a Function of Teaching, Delegation, and Supervision

Teaching

Close interaction of experienced anesthesia professionals with inexperienced clinical trainees during actual surgical procedures is a standard approach to training. It can be hypothesized that teaching adds to the workload of the more experienced care provider who is simultaneously responsible for safe and efficient anesthesia care during the procedure. Weinger and co-workers found that teaching teams, involving one-to-one supervision of fourth-year medical students or first-month anesthesia residents by an attending anesthesiologist, had significantly slower response times to a warning light than non-teaching teams of attending(s) of similar experience. Response latency was highest during induction and emergence. This vigilance test was also a procedural (performance) workload assessment measure indicating increased workload and reduced spare capacity. They also found that workload density was significantly increased for teaching as opposed to non-teaching teams. In sum, intraoperative teaching increased workload and decreased vigilance, suggesting the need for caution when educating during patient care.

Delegation and Supervision

Experience suggests that the effect of delegation on workload varies depending on the nature of the task and how confident the delegating anesthesia professional feels about the capability of the person to whom the task is assigned. Delegation must be guided by the supervisor’s situation awareness and overall ability to process information on the patients’ condition. Several further aspects are presented in the work of Leedal and Smith.

Performance as a Function of Experience

Routine Events

Novice trainee anesthesia professionals were found to perform many of the same tasks as do more experienced personnel at specific phases of an anesthetic regimen, but take longer over tasks, show longer latency of response, and greater task workload than third-year trainees and experienced nurse anesthetists. While task density was generally highest for both subject groups in the period up to, during, and immediately after intubation, the more experienced trainees had higher task densities than the novices, suggesting greater competence among the former in carrying out multiple tasks in a short period of time. Those findings are in line with other studies, including the study of Weinger and associates that evaluated the mean response time of pressing a buzzer at the flashing of a red light (secondary task). The response time was markedly less than 60 seconds for experienced subjects in both the induction and post induction (maintenance) phases, but it was much higher for novice residents during the induction phase. One explanation for those findings may be that the reduction of workload depends partly on the degree to which tasks can become routine, thus freeing mental resources for other tasks. Leedal and Smith conclude concerning this matter in their review: “Experienced staff appear to show ‘spare capacity’ in performance during routine cases, which we suggest allows them an attentional ‘safety margin’ should adverse events occur ” (p. 708)

In contrast, a more recent study presented by Byrne and co-workers found there was limited evidence of a relationship between workload and experience.

Another recent finding indicates that more experienced anesthesia teams may be more likely to attempt to coordinate implicitly, without much overt communication, which makes them more reliant on accurate and shared understandings of the task and their teamwork.

Novice residents also spent more time speaking to their attending staff (11% of preintubation time) than did experienced residents or CRNAs. Experienced personnel observed the surgical field more than did the novices. Novices did take longer to complete patient preparation and induction of anesthesia, but it appeared that some of the extra time taken by novices working under supervision was offset by the efficiency of offloading other concurrent tasks to the attending anesthesiologist such that preintubation time was increased by only 6 minutes for novices.

Critical Events/Emergencies

Schulz and colleagues presented data where more experienced anesthetists (>2 years work experience) increased the amount of time dedicated to manual tasks from 21% to 25% during critical incidents, whereas the less experienced decreased from 20% to 14%. The less experienced anesthesia providers spend more time on monitoring tasks.

A study by Byrne and Jones looked at differences in the performance of experienced and less experienced anesthesia professionals during 180 simulated anesthesia emergency scenarios. The results showed significant differences only between the first and second year. As seen in other studies, significant errors occurred at all levels of experience, and most of the anesthesia professionals deviated from established guidelines.

A classic simulation study by DeAnda and Gaba investigated the response of anesthesia trainees and experienced anesthesia faculty and private practitioners to six preplanned critical incidents of differing type and severity. The incidents included (1) endobronchial intubation (EI) resulting from surgical manipulation of the tube; (2) occlusion of intravenous (IV) tubing; (3) atrial fibrillation (AF) with a rapid ventricular response and hypotension; (4) airway disconnection between the endotracheal tube and the breathing circuit; (5) breathing hoses too short to turn the table 180 degrees, as requested by the surgeon; and (6) ventricular tachycardia or fibrillation. For each incident, considerable interindividual variability was found in detection and correction times, in information sources used, and in actions taken. The average performance of the anesthesia professionals tended to improve with experience, although this varied by incident. The performance of the experienced groups was not better than that of the second-year residents (who were in their final year of training at that time). Many (but not all) novice residents performed indistinguishably from more experienced subjects. Each experience group contained some who required excessive time to solve the problem or who never solved it. In each experience group at least one individual made major errors that could have had a substantial negative impact on a patient’s clinical outcome. For example, one faculty member never used electrical countershock to treat ventricular fibrillation. One private practitioner treated the EI as though it were “bronchospasm” and never assessed the symmetry of ventilation. One resident never found the airway disconnection. The elements of suboptimal performance were both technical and cognitive. Technical problems included choosing defibrillation energies appropriate for internal paddles when using external paddles, ampule swap, and failure to inflate the endotracheal tube cuff that resulted in a leak. Cognitive problems included failure to allocate attention to the most critical problems and fixation errors.

Schwid and O’Donnell performed an experiment similar to those of DeAnda and Gaba and received similar results. After working on several practice cases without critical incidents, each subject was asked to manage 3 or 4 cases involving a total of 4 serious critical events (esophageal intubation, myocardial ischemia, anaphylaxis, and cardiac arrest). The anesthesiologists studied had varying experience levels. Significant errors in diagnosis or treatment were made in every experience group. The errors occurred in both diagnosis of problems and in deciding on and implementing appropriate treatment. For example, 60% of subjects did not make the diagnosis of anaphylaxis despite available information on heart rate, blood pressure, wheezing, increased peak inspiratory pressure, and the presence of a rash. In managing myocardial ischemia, multiple failures occurred. 30% of subjects did not compensate for severe abnormalities while considering diagnostic maneuvers. Fixation errors in which initial diagnoses and plans were never revised were frequent, even when they were clearly wrong.

Independent from professionals’ experience, Howard and colleagues found a substantial incidence of difficulties during simulated emergencies: managing multiple problems simultaneously, applying attention to the most critical needs, acting as team leader, communicating with personnel, and using all available OR resources to best advantage. Analysis of videotaped trauma and resuscitation cases has revealed inadequacies in the availability and arrangement of monitoring equipment, as well as nonexistent or ambiguous communication. Byrne and Jones evaluated the performance of anesthetists during nine emergency cases, showing that serious errors in both diagnosis and treatment were made and accepted treatment guidelines were not followed. Diagnosis of several common critical incidents, for example, anaphylaxis, was hard to name. As described earlier, once diagnosed, no structured plans or algorithms were used.

These classic studies date back 25 years or more, yet the most recent studies remain completely consistent with older studies. In the 2017 Weinger and colleagues publication, a total of 263 consenting U.S. board-certified anesthesiologists (BCAs—i.e., physicians) participated in two, 20-minute, standardized, high-fidelity simulation scenarios of unanticipated acute events during existing Maintenance of Certification in Anesthesiology simulation courses. The scenarios were from among the following: (1) local anesthetic systemic toxicity with hemodynamic collapse; (2) hemorrhagic shock due to hidden retroperitoneal bleeding during laparoscopy; (3) malignant hyperthermia presenting in the postanesthesia care unit; and (4) acute onset of AF with hemodynamic instability during laparotomy followed by ST elevation myocardial infarction. Performance measurement rubrics were established in advance: a scoresheet of critical clinical performance elements, behavioral anchored ordinal scales of four nontechnical skills, ordinal scales for overall individual and team technical performance and nontechnical performance, and a binary rating as to whether performance met or exceeded that expected of a BCA. The results showed that critical clinical performance elements were commonly omitted, roughly in four broad areas of crisis management, failures to: (1) escalate therapy where first-line options did not work (e.g., using epinephrine or vasopressin when phenylephrine, ephedrine, or fluids did not sufficiently correct hypotension); (2) use available resources (e.g., calling for help when conditions have deteriorated appreciably); (3) speak up and engage other team members, especially when action by them was required (e.g., asking the surgeon to change the surgical approach when it is essential to effective treatment); and (4) follow evidence-based guidelines (e.g., giving dantrolene to a patient with obvious MH). The performance of approximately 25% of subjects was rated in the low portion of the various technical and nontechnical 9-point ordinal scales. In about 30% of encounters, performance was rated as “below the level expected of a BCA.”

Patient Safety Action Box

Experienced anesthetists are not immune to error. Studies show that significant errors can occur at all levels of experience. Experience is not a substitute for excellence or expertise , making recurrent training and continuous awareness of possible safety pitfalls important, independent of the experience level.

Patient Safety Action Box

Do not think this cannot happen to you… In her paper, “Lake Wobegon for anesthesia...where everyone is above average except those who aren’t: variability in the management of simulated intraoperative critical incidents,” McIntosh discusses the applicability to anesthesiology of the pervasive human tendency to overestimate one’s achievements and capabilities in relation to others. Such effects have been documented for drivers, CEOs, stock market analysts, college students, parents, and state education officials, among others. Indeed we may all be living in Lake Wobegon in terms of evaluating our own abilities to manage critical events.

What are the practical implications of performance assessment in Anesthesia?

In summary, simulation studies on performance all showed appreciable performance gaps of anesthesia professionals managing critical incidents and emergencies. The results expand on the few comparable data on real cases. Overall, the surprisingly high frequency of mediocre performance represents a patient safety concern for the field of anesthesiology, emphasizing the need for anesthesia professionals and their organizations to:

  • be aware of performance shortcomings and pitfalls in everyday work

  • regard the knowledge and training of performance-enhancing strategies as one of the core competencies

  • focus on the application of and adherence to evidence-based practice guidelines

  • focus on the efficient management of the environment (team, resources, equipment, etc.)

  • implement case management skills in the education, training, and re-certification, that go beyond only medical/technical knowledge and skill

Benefits and Challenges of Assessing Performance

Benefits of the Scientific Study of Tasks and Performance in Anesthesia

The generation of an improved understanding of the human performance of anesthesia professionals can help them to provide patient care more safely, in a wider variety of clinical situations, with greater efficiency, and with increased satisfaction to both patients and practitioners. The possible benefits of studying human performance include, but are not limited to, the following:

  • 1.

    Improved clinical performance: Behavior observation helps to identify which processes and behaviors are associated with effective and safe performance. Thus, it provides us with new knowledge on what effective teams do differently compared with ineffective teams and how they do it. Compared with self-reports, behavior observation allows for measuring actual team-level phenomena and dynamics that teams may not even be aware of and that may unfold over time.

  • 2.

    Improved operational protocols: The way in which individuals conduct anesthesia is based, in part, on knowing the limits of their performance envelope. Anesthetic techniques and OR practices should draw on anesthesia professionals’ abilities and should mitigate their weaknesses.

  • 3.

    Enhanced clinical education and training of anesthesia professionals: Understanding the required performance characteristics and inherent human limitations will lead to improved training, which will most fully develop the strengths and counter the existing vulnerabilities of the anesthesia professional. Identification of performance gaps informs opportunities for improvement. Identification of expert knowledge through elicitation of unaware expertise behavior creates educational gain. Taking this knowledge into action should make patient care safer, less stressful, and more efficient.

  • 4.

    A more effective work environment: Anesthesia professionals now perform their tasks by using an array of technologies, many of which have not been designed to support the anesthesia professional’s work optimally. By understanding the relevant tasks and performance requirements, the workspace and tools could be improved for better support of the most difficult tasks. This, too, can lead to greater safety and to greater efficiency and work satisfaction.

  • 5.

    A more efficient organizational system: Anesthesiology is embedded within a larger system of organized medical care that involves interactions among numerous people, institutions, organizations, and professional domains. Understanding how the anesthesia professional’s work relates to the larger system may enable the development of more rational and efficient flow of information and organizational control.

  • 6.

    A more rational view of professional work and legal responsibility: Modern health care, especially in the United States, is strongly influenced by medicolegal concerns. The litigation system has a major selection bias in that every case that comes before it involves an adverse outcome for a patient. The duty of the practitioner is to render care as a reasonable and prudent specialist in the area of anesthesia. What is reasonable and prudent? What type of performance is to be expected from appropriately trained human beings in a complex and dynamic environment? By understanding human performance, it may be possible to generate a more rational view of what is and is not within the standard of care.

Challenges of the Scientific Study of Tasks and Performance in Anesthesia

Study of human performance involves research paradigms different from those typically used in the science of anesthesia. Many obstacles exist to obtaining valid data on human performance. There are no animal models for expert human performance and no Sprague-Dawley anesthesia professionals to be studied in detail. Recruiting experienced personnel to be the subjects of study is difficult and raises issues of selection bias concerning those who do volunteer. Especially if conducted during actual patient care, investigations of human performance are strongly influenced by concerns about litigation, credentialing, and confidentiality, thus making it difficult to execute optimal studies. Furthermore, variability among individual anesthesia professionals is quite striking because different anesthesia professionals respond to the same situation in different ways, and each individual may act differently on different days or at different times of the same day. The magnitude of this intraindividual variability is often nearly the same as the interindividual variability. Another challenge is that the performance measures may not be sensitive to an increase or decrease in workload if the subject compensates through increased, or reduced, effort respectively.

Performance itself is an intuitively meaningful concept that is difficult to define precisely. No universal standards are available for the clinical decisions and actions of anesthesia professionals. They depend heavily on the context of specific situations. In addition, determining how anesthesia professionals perform their jobs, whether successfully or unsuccessfully, means delving into their mental processes. This cannot be measured easily. Experimental designs can involve artificial laboratory tasks for which performance can be objectively measured, but these tasks will then be far removed from the real world of administering anesthesia. Conversely, investigating the actual performance of trained practitioners in the real world yields primarily subjective and indirect data. Understanding the anesthesia professional’s performance must be seen as analogous to solving a jigsaw puzzle, an analogy introduced by Gaba and extended by McIntosh. Pieces of the puzzle probably come from a variety of sources, none of which by itself captures the entire picture.

Problems faced by all investigators are the lack of an accepted standard for objective or subjective evaluation of anesthesia professional performance and the absence of an agreed-on methodology for analyzing and describing anesthesia professional performance. As one result of the divergent research landscape in human performance, the number, scope, and variety of applied behavior observation taxonomies are growing, making comparison and convergent integration of research findings difficult. Several groups were working on methodologies for evaluating technical and behavioral aspects of performance.

Kolbe and colleagues pointed out four methodological challenges when rating behavior. First is identifying the optimal balance between specificity versus generalizability. Researchers must decide whether to investigate processes from a general perspective using methods that capture all teamwork behaviors simultaneously or whether to focus on one single process (e.g., closed-loop communication) to explore it in detail. Second is deciding whether to rate the quality or describe the occurrence of teamwork behavior. Quality of behavior can be measured with an anchored ordinal rating scale (e.g., from excellent to poor). Or alternatively, the occurrences of particular behaviors can be measured such as when?—by whom?—to whom?, etc., delivering different results. Third is linking research findings with team training content, when no common language and no common behavioral codes, respectively, are used in research. Fourth is applying different rating systems (in different studies) without respect to the usability and the different requirements for research, training, and examination purposes.

Patient Safety on the Individual and Team Level

In earlier sections the characteristics of the complex and dynamic working environment in anesthesia were described in detail, as well as the multiple manual, behavioral, and cognitive tasks and related performance assessment studies of the anesthesia professional. Both sections illustrated several human performance and patient safety challenges that personnel face regularly. The knowledge and management of the complex work environment and the various tasks, workload, and performance pitfalls can determine the ultimate success of professionals’ intervention. These human factors comprise issues related to perception, memory, problem solving, physiological rhythm, and more. This section focusses on the so-called nontechnical skills and performance-shaping factors as they have the most direct practical impact for the work of anesthesia professionals.

The importance of nontechnical skills and performance-shaping factors is also in line with the performance studies of anesthesiologists mentioned earlier in this chapter. They revealed room for improvement at any level of experience, especially in non-routine and emergency situations. Recent views on nontechnical skills emphasize their relevance also in routine situations to prevent those from becoming critical. Nontechnical skills explain difficulties in applying knowledge and skills that team members possess in stressful moments in a dynamic, complex, and high-workload environment. They are related to challenges in managing and coordinating oneself (e.g., remembering to do tasks, monitoring one’s own actions, acting as a team leader), the team (e.g., distributing tasks, managing conflict, sharing mental models), and the equipment (e.g., knowing the application of the equipment, understanding different use modes, troubleshooting).

Therefore, in the upcoming sections the following topics are addressed: (1) General concepts of human factors (HF) and nontechnical skills (NTS) are introduced and discussed in the larger context of human performance. Subsequently, several examples of the impact of HF/NTS are given, indicating that from a safety point of view, HFs and NTS deserve as much attention as medical knowledge [patho-] physiology, diagnosis, treatment) and practical skills, which by tradition have dominated training programs for anesthesia professionals. (2) Two sets of key elements of patient safety: first relating to individual performance (situation awareness and decision making), and second relating to team performance (communication, teamwork, and task management). (3) Additionally, individual performance-shaping factors are discussed, in particular fatigue, interruptions, distractions, and ambient noise.

Human Performance, Human Factors, and NonTechnical Skills

In the literature, the interrelated terms human performance, human factors, and nontechnical skills are used in a variety of ways and sometimes even synonymously, making it difficult to classify them. They are interconnected and while several models and taxonomies exist there is not always a clear distinction between these terms and concepts. In the upcoming section the authors explain the general underlying principles and give a simplified overview of their interrelationships in order to generate a more systematic understanding of key concepts. The term human error, also used in this context occasionally, is itself a different term, almost the flipside of human factors. One might say that challenges in human factors (and other aspects) can result in human error. For the definition and classification of human error see later section on “System Thinking.”

Human Performance and Human Factors

What are Human Factors? Human performance is shaped, positively or negatively, by different levels of so-called human factors (HF). Good HF increases and poor HF decreases human performance. The term ergonomics is also used in some contexts. A broad variety of disciplines are involved and many different topics are embraced by both terms; hence, several definitions exist. The Human Factors and Ergonomics Society defines HF as follows: “Human factors is concerned with the application of what we know about people, their abilities, characteristics, and limitations to the design of equipment they use, environments in which they function, and jobs they perform.” Catchpole and McCulloch define human factors in the medical context as: “Enhancing clinical performance through an understanding of the effects of teamwork, tasks, equipment, workspace, culture, and organization on human behavior and abilities and application of that knowledge in clinical settings.”

Different Components of Human Factors

Derived from the SEIPS model and adapted by the authors, different components of HF are:

  • the behavior of individuals and their behavior/knowledge in regard to tasks (individual level)

  • the interactions with each other (team level)

  • the interactions of professionals with the organizational/sociocultural conditions (organizational level)

  • the interactions of professionals with the environment/workspace (environmental level) and

  • the interactions of professionals with technology/equipment (technology/engineering/design level)

Those five human factor components are necessary and sufficient to describe and understand the anesthesia professional’s entire work system from a HF perspective. The components interact with and influence each other, resulting in a large number of relationships between different levels. At times the components compensate each other (e.g., when professionals work faster to compensate for time pressure or when people collaborate to solve problems that are beyond an individual’s abilities). Other times the components resonate with each other and amplify their effects—for the good (e.g., having the right equipment for the task at hand) or the bad (e.g., lacking resources to solve a problem). Given that this chapter cannot deal with human factors in a comprehensive way, many topics can be touched on only briefly. Although the environmental and technology levels can be important—cognition is challenged when there is a power failure or a breakdown of key clinical equipment—in this chapter the focus lies on the most important aspects of human factors directly relevant to anesthesia professionals: the individual level, team level, and organizational level.

Human Factors and Nontechnical Skills

What are NonTechnical skills in comparison to Human Factors? When talking specifically about HF that are directly related to actions of a single individual and/or of a team, often this is referred to as the concept of nontechnical skills (NTS). NTS are defined as “the cognitive, social, and personal resource skills that complement technical skills (which encompass the technical knowledge of health care and its various procedures), and contribute to safe and efficient task performance” or alternatively as “attitudes and behaviors not directly related to the use of medical expertise, drugs, or equipment.”

While some authors object to the term nontechnical skills—using a negative to describe something—others have pointed out that not only is this term in wide use already, but that actually a variety of terms involving negation are in common use in science, mathematics, and medicine. One main paper and an editorial discuss this issue. For health care, instead of nontechnical skills, the term could also be non-medical skills. However, because of the extensive use of nontechnical skills in the literature of other industries and in health care, this chapter uses that terminology.

Different Ways to Categorize NonTechnical Skills

In general, NTS can be categorized into two broad areas: (1) cognitive and mental skills on the individual level, including decision making and situation awareness; and (2) social and interpersonal skills on the team level, including teamwork, communication, and leadership. Commercial aviation incorporated such nontechnical skills in the cockpit (later crew) resource management paradigm (CRM, 1980s and continuous later evolution) and the “NOTECHS” paradigm in the late 1990s. In anesthesiology, the anesthesia crisis resource management (ACRM) framework was introduced by Howard and co-workers in 1990 as an adaptation of aviation’s CRM. The ACRM approach categorizes NTS based on the five key elements of communication, situation awareness, decision making, teamwork (implicitly including leadership), and task management ( Fig. 6.5 ). The anesthesia nontechnical skills (ANTS) framework by Fletcher and colleagues was introduced in 2003; Flin and colleagues give an overview of its history, development, application, use, and emerging is issues. , The ANTS approach typically includes the four categories of situation awareness, decision making, teamwork (explicitly including leadership), and task management. Neither of these frameworks, and indeed no usable paradigm, can capture explicitly every important aspect of HF applied to anesthesiology. For example the management of the performance-shaping factors stress and fatigue are also part of the ANTS framework, but are implicit in ACRM. Communication itself is not an explicit skill element of the ANTS framework (which assumes that communication pervades each element), whereas in ACRM it is a specific skill that needs explicit mention and training. Of note, from their start in anesthesiology, nontechnical skills frameworks have been further adapted to several other medical fields, such as for surgeons and intensive care specialists.

Fig. 6.5, The five main elements of Crisis Resource Management (CRM).

Assessment of NonTechnical Skills

In response to the growing acceptance of human factors and nontechnical skills influencing medical performance, several rubrics to measure NTS have been developed. Gaba and colleagues in 1998 described the direct adaptation for assessment of NTS of a set of CRM-anchored ordinal scale markers from Helmreich et al. concerning aviation. In 2004 the ANTS framework was complemented by a behaviorally anchored rating scale in 2004. A new set of assessment scales for four markers of NTS was introduced by Weinger and colleagues in 2017 based on the fusion of aspects of a number of prior approaches.

A further, rather new approach for the assessment of nontechnical skills is the Co-ACT framework described by Kolbe and co-workers. The framework serves observing coordination behavior, especially in acute care teams like anesthesia, and consists of four categories, each category obtaining three further subelements that more specifically describe the NTS: (1) explicit action coordination with the elements instruction, speaking up , and planning ; (2) implicit action coordination with the elements monitoring , talking to the room (action-related), and providing assistance ; (3) explicit information coordination with the elements information request , information evaluation , and information upon request ; and (4) implicit information coordination with the elements gathering information, talking to the room (information-related), and information without request .

Challenges of the Assessment of NonTechnical Skills

The psychometric qualities of measuring NTS have been assessed by the developers of the ANTS system and were considered to be of acceptable level, whereas another study assessed the reliability after a 1-day training for raters and concluded that reliability was poor. A study from Denmark showed good psychometric qualities. Originally developed for educational purposes and used to discuss the NTS of anesthesiologists after training sessions in order to improve in NTS, Zwaan and colleagues assessed the usability and reliability for ANTS in research, finding that the ANTS system was reliable for the total score and usable to measure physicians’ NTS in a research setting. However, the investigators found a variation between the reliability of the different elements and recommend excluding elements in advance that are not applicable or observable in the situation of interest. It might not always be easy, however, to identify those elements that should be excluded. On the whole, the ANTS system appears to be a useful tool to enhance assessment of nontechnical skills in anesthesia and other medical fields further, and its careful derivation from an established system of nontechnical assessment in aviation (NOTECHS) may even allow some interdomain comparisons. However, more recently Watkins and colleagues directly compared ANTS with the system used in the Weinger and colleagues 2017 paper and showed that ANTS was more difficult to use but that the two systems otherwise achieved equivalent assessment results.

NonTechnical Skills: The Bad - The Good - The Variable

Good nontechnical skills (i.e., vigilance, efficient communication, team coordination, etc.) reduce the likelihood of active and passive errors and adverse events, and also increase human performance, whereas suboptimal NTS are expected to do the reverse. These effects are variable; for example, a person may communicate very effectively in one instance and fail to do so in the next challenging communication role.

A study from Denmark investigated the relationship between technical and nontechnical skills. Twenty-five video recordings of second-year anesthesiologists managing a simulated difficult airway management scenario were rated with the ANTS instrument and a score for the technical aspect for the procedure. In addition, written descriptions of the NTS performance were collected and content analyzed. The correlation between the two scores was not significant, but the content analysis comparing the NTS description in the best and poorest three scenarios identified what contributed most to good NTS. These were systematically collecting information, thinking ahead, communicating and justifying of decisions, delegating tasks, and vigilantly responding to the evolving situation. Poor NTS were related to: lack of structured approach, lack of articulating plans and decisions, poor resource and task management, lack of considering consequences of treatment, poor response to the evolving situation, and lack of leadership.

What is the Impact of Non-Technical Skills and Human Factors on Poor Performance in Medicine?

The impact of poor NTS on poor human performance in the medical field cannot be overestimated. Depending on the literature, it is estimated that up to 80% of all errors in medicine can be attributed to problems with NTS and human factors.

Even though one can argue on the one hand that due to progress in technology the findings and estimates of the pioneer study of Cooper and colleagues performed in 1978 (results relating up to 80% of incidents to human factors) have been outdated for a long time, on the other hand those figures are (1) comparable to findings in other dynamic and complex work environments and (2) newer studies as recent as 2015 still reconfirm those findings (references see below). In the following some illustrative studies are mentioned that link HF and safety challenges.

In 1993 an analysis of 2000 incident reports from the Australian Incident Reporting Study took place, investigating the incidents for relations to human factors and NTS. In 83% of incidents, human factor elements were scored by the reporters. Even though scoring by the incident reporters might not produce results as accurate as those of a systematic scoring by human factor experts and such text descriptions are of limited value for this type of data collection, it still shows the scope of the problem—having also in mind that voluntarily reported incidents only represent the peak of an iceberg, with many incidents not reported at all.

Fletcher and colleagues published a review of studies describing the influence of nontechnical skills in anesthesia in 2002, summarizing that it is clear “ that nontechnical skills play a central role in good anesthesia practice and that a wide range of behaviors are important […] [including] monitoring, allocation of attention, planning and preparation, situation awareness, prioritization, applying predefined strategies/protocols, flexibility in decision-making, communication, and team-working” [p.426].

As increasing evidence suggested that human factors like communication, leadership, and team interaction influence the performance of cardiopulmonary resuscitation (CPR), Hunziker and colleagues presented a study in 2010 reviewing the impact of human factors during simulation-based resuscitation scenarios. Similar to studies in real patients, simulated cardiac arrest scenarios revealed many unnecessary interruptions of CPR as well as significant delays in defibrillation—two outcome-relevant parameters. The studies showed that human factors played a major role in these shortcomings and that medical performance, at least in non-routine situations, depends among others on the quality of leadership and team-structuring.

Jones and co-workers only recently systematically reviewed the literature on the impact of HFs in preventing complications in anesthesia due to poor airway management and highlighted recent national reports and guidelines, including the 4th National Audit Project (NAP4). NAP4 (2011) was the first prospective study of all major airway events occurring throughout the UK, reviewing any complications resulting from airway management that led to either death, brain damage, the need for an emergency surgical airway, unanticipated ICU admission, or prolongation of ICU stay. 184 reports were reviewed. Subsequent in-depth analysis identified HFs as having been a relevant influence in every case, with a median range of 4.5 contributing HFs per case. HFs in the report included for example : casual attitude toward risk/overconfidence, peer tolerance of poor standards, lack of clarity in team structures, poor or dysfunctional communication including incomplete or inadequate handovers, inadequate checking procedures, failure to formulate back-up plans and discuss them with team members, failure to use available equipment, attempts to use unknown equipment in an emergency situation, heavy personal workload, lack of time to undertake thorough assessment, equipment shortage, inexperienced personnel working unsupervised, organizational cultures which induce or tolerate unsafe practices, no formalized requirement to undertake checking procedures, incompatible goals, and reluctance to analyze adverse events and learn from errors.

The Sentinel Event Report of the Joint Commission analyzed the root causes of 764 sentinel events (patient’s death, loss in function, unexpected additional care and/or psychological impact) reported voluntarily in 2014. Human-factor-related root causes, including among others communication and leadership, were the leading root causes of sentinel events and made up 65% of root causes. Even though the data are not specifically linked to anesthesia practice, it is very likely that there exist strong parallels.

One of the latest simulation-based studies, performed by Weinger and colleagues in 2017 and explained in detail earlier (section on assessing performance, “Performance as a Function of Experience”), also shows a broad variety of human-factor-related hurdles for anesthesiologists when handling emergencies.

What are the implications of the Impact of Humand Factors and NonTechnical Skills on Human Performance?

  • Balancing the importance of technical and nontechnical skills during medical education and training. While technical skills and medical knowledge have always been at the core of medical education and training, the importance of NTS has—despite long growing evidence—only recently been recognized.

  • Acknowledgement of the importance of human factors as individual and team. One of the first steps for individuals and teams in minimizing and mitigating human frailties and strengthening human strengths—and in consequence reducing medical error and its consequences—is by acknowledging human factors as a part of human performance and thus acknowledging human limitation. The World Health Organization (WHO) states:

“A failure to apply human factors principles is a key aspect of most adverse events in health care. Therefore, all health-care workers need to have a basic understanding of human factors principles. Health-care workers who do not understand the basics of human factors are like infection control professionals not knowing about microbiology.” (p. 111)

Human Factors on the Individual Level

This section will introduce three levels of human factors in more detail: (1) understanding the behavior of individuals (individual level), (2) the interactions between individuals (team level), and (3) their interactions with the organization (organizational level). This first section on the individual level addresses five interrelated elements: (1a) task management, (1b) situation awareness, (1c) decision making, (1d) general individual performance-shaping factors (e.g., fatigue, distractions and noise), and (1e) personal (safety) attitudes.

Task Management

According to the ANTS framework, task management is defined as (p. 8) “skills for organizing resources and required activities to achieve goals, be they individual case plans or longer-term scheduling issues. It has four skill elements: planning and preparing; prioritizing; providing and maintaining standards; identifying and utilizing resources .

Anesthesia professionals are often—within the operating room setting—their own phlebotomist, IV inserter, echocardiographer, pharmacist, technician, cleaner, data recorder, patient transporter, as well as performer of activities on behalf of the surgeon (e.g., manipulate the OR table, answer telephone calls, etc.) all while maintaining situation awareness, engaging in decision making, doing administrative tasks, and managing the anesthetic itself. That’s a lot of activities! Task management in a team might include the delegation of tasks to appropriate personnel, using effective communication, and closing the loop, both to verify the request was received and understood and then to inform the leader that the task is done, or that there are problems.

Task density can become so high that (1) errors in tasks or their subtasks are more likely; (2) no single person can manage all the tasks alone so that help from team members is needed (teamwork); and (3) tasks may be delayed by interruptions or competing demands, which then require prospective memory to remember what to do in the future or to correctly resume interrupted actions.

Multitasking and Multiplexing

Sometimes multitasking—doing several things at a time—can be successful, depending on the situation and the tasks at hand. Yet professionals need to be aware that it can be unsuccessful when the situation or the tasks change. When task density becomes high, simple linear performance of tasks becomes impossible; instead more complicated processes of both multitasking and multiplexing are needed. Multitasking—trying to perform two (or more) tasks simultaneously—is frequent in hospital settings, but is virtually impossible to execute if the same cognitive resources are involved. Conducting two or more relatively simple tasks is often possible, such as observing the surgical field while asking the surgeon a straightforward question. Even then there is a risk that one task will be dropped or degraded. However, adjusting an infusion pump while calculating the upcoming dose of antibiotic for an infant would be difficult to do in parallel.

In common use, the term multitasking sometimes refers specifically to media multitasking—everyday attempts to simultaneously work, listen to music, check email and text messages, and search the web. Studies have shown that frequent media multitaskers perform worse on psychology laboratory probes of actual multitasking than those who are infrequent media multitaskers. Many suggest that true multitasking is impossible and that almost always there is a decrement of performance on some or all tasks when multitasking is attempted.

The degree to which media multitasking, or laboratory tests, relate to the kinds of multitasking performed by personnel in professional settings of many data streams and many tasks is as yet uncertain; it seems likely that in anesthesia care there are limits to what individuals can safely do on their own.

While simple multitasking in perioperative settings may work, when tasks are complex it may in fact be inefficient. Rather than conducting two (or more) activities in parallel, humans often do things sequentially but shift rapidly between items or back and forth across many simultaneous threads. If the tasks use different cognitive resources, this is referred to as multiplexing. Multiplexing brings the challenges that people (1) have to refocus their concentration each time they switch and (2) are more susceptible to distractions and errors. This is especially true if the tasks at hand involve intensive real-time control as opposed to being cognitively automatic. There is evidence that health care personnel are not aware of the risks of decreased performance when attempting to multitask.

Douglas and co-authors recently reviewed the current literature concerning multitasking in the health care setting. They suggest that multitasking typically results in increased time of task completion, increased stress, risk of memory lapses, and subsequent errors and accidents. Those performance limitations occur more often if one is forced to multitask by the environment and/or if the different tasks compete for the same local cognitive resource. Their review located only two studies on the association between multitasking and errors in the health care setting although nothing definitive was found. Executing two tasks at the same time was found to carry the risk of decreased accuracy and efficiency and to have an increased reaction time to environmental stimuli.

Patient Safety Action Box

Keep focused. Instead of trying to do several things at once—and maybe none of them well—shift your attention consciously and completely from one task to the next and organize the tasks at hand according to priority. Giving your full attention to what you are doing will help you do it better, with fewer mistakes and less load on your prospective memory. Prevent or modulate avoidable distractions whenever possible.

Multitasking During Handovers?

One study observed different modes of patient handover from the OR to the post-anesthesia recovery unit in six hospitals. The researchers compared handovers of (1) simultaneous transfer of equipment and information (i.e., a nurse connecting the monitor while at the same time receiving verbal information) to (2) sequential transfer (equipment is first connected and lines sorted, then verbal information follows). The findings from 101 observed handovers showed that 65% of them took place simultaneously. Interestingly, simultaneous handovers were not significantly faster than sequential ones (1.8 vs. 2.0 min). In review of postoperative handovers Segall and colleagues recommended: (1) standardize/structure processes (e.g., through the use of checklists and protocols); (2) complete urgent clinical tasks before the information transfer; (3) allow only patient-specific discussions during verbal handovers; (4) require that all relevant team members be present; and (5) provide training in team skills and communication. More information on handoffs is given in the later section on “Communication.”

Patient Safety Action Box

The recommendations given above are helpful for safe postoperative handovers. Another handover type occurs when one anesthesia professional replaces another, temporarily or permanently. And yet another handover is when you have called someone for help or when you are the one coming to help. For this last situation often the person arriving to help concentrates immediately on performing tasks without really listening to the briefing from the original anesthesia professional. Unless the task is critical (e.g., CPR) it is better to wait, assess the situation and the observable information, and then pay full attention to the briefing. For further information on handoffs see section “Effective Communication and Delegation of Tasks.”

Situation Awareness

In anesthesia many sources of information need to be scanned in order to get the overview of the situation. These include, but are not limited to: the patient (clinical impression and history), various monitoring equipment, the patient chart, explicit and implicit information by colleagues and OR team members, and workday characteristics (i.e., staff composition, staff shortage, available equipment/time, etc.). Gaba, Howard, and Small introduced the concept of SA in the context of anesthesia in 1995. Schulz and colleagues wrote a review article in 2013 especially on SA in anesthesia.

Situation awareness (SA) describes “the ability of an individual to maintain an adequate internal representation of the status of the environment in complex and dynamic domains where time constants are short and conditions may change within seconds and minutes” (p. 729).

SA is a concept drawn from military aviation concerning a variety of aspects of cognition in complex real-time environments. While important itself, situation awareness is influenced by previous decisions, communication patterns, and team dynamics; the level of SA in turn influences team communication and coordination. Situation awareness incorporates the circumstances of the patient at hand (e.g., “Is all required expertise represented?”), the state of the treatment team (e.g., “Is a team member task-overloaded?,” “Do team members trust each other?”); and the state of the environment including the rest of the facility (e.g., “Is the problem I’m having also happening elsewhere in my OR suite?”).

One of the primary experts on SA, Mica Endsley, postulates three different components of individual SA: (1) Perception of the elements of a situation/environment within a volume of time and space (= gathering information, detection of cues); (2) comprehension of elements and their meaning (= interpretation of information/cues, diagnosis); and (3) projection of their status in the near future (= anticipation, prediction). Dekker and Hollnagel challenge this model.

Success or failure of SA can occur at each component: gathering, comprehension, and projecting. Some readily available information can be missed, or perceived incorrectly; on the other hand experience makes them sensitive to barely perceptible cues. They also often, but not always, comprehend the meaning of what is perceived by everyone even when others miss its significance. Experts also are usually able to assess the situation not only as it stands but also to project where things are headed, but of course their future projection can be inaccurate or disregarded (including the “everything is OK fixation error”).

Schulz and colleagues reviewed 200 incidents in anesthesia and critical care in an in-hospital setting and determined the frequency of situation awareness errors for an individual patient. Admittedly there could be some degree of hindsight bias; nonetheless, situation awareness errors were identified in 81.5% of cases—predominantly in perception (38.0%) and comprehension (31.5%).

Shared Situation Awareness

SA is a concept that can be applied to the individual (personal SA, see above), but also the team (shared SA). Team SA is defined as “the degree to which every team member possesses the SA required for his or her responsibilities” (p. 39). Within different professions and different physical positions in the room, the situational awareness and interpretation of the same case can differ substantially within a team, due to variation in experience, involvement, focus point, interest, knowledge, or information. Of course, not every bit of information can be shared with the whole team; doing so would overwhelm everyone. Yet, creating and maintaining an overall shared mental model of the situation across all team members is critical for determining the best plan of action for patient care. Some team communication in the operating room that seemingly does not serve an obvious purpose, might, in fact, be a gauging of shared mental models and therefore shared SA. Anesthesia professionals, for example, routinely assess the progress and possible complications of the operation by perceiving nuances in the conversation of the surgeons.

Shared mental models predict good team performance and were, for example, shown to be related to positive performance in trauma teams, with team leaders and followers actively working toward an information exchange. Adapted from psychology and management literature, Schmutz and Eppich introduced the conceptual framework of team reflexivity to health care. They propose that shared SA is fostered by components of team reflexivity, namely (1) pre-action briefing, (2) in-action deliberation, and (3) post-action debriefing. Fioratou and colleagues challenge the common model of individual and team SA and instead introduced the model of distributed situation awareness (DSA) to anesthesia, arguing that cognition actually involves elements of the (physical) environment, for example, the values on a monitor.

A multicenter study from Denmark explored shared mental models of surgical teams during 64 video-assisted thoracoscopic surgery lobectomies to explore familiarity of the people involved in the operation with each other; mutual assessment of technical and nontechnical skills in the other persons present during the operation; assessment of the perceived risk for the patient from the procedure and from the anesthetic; and noting of problems in the present co-works task management. There was poor agreement between team members’ risk ratings showing limitations in how much the members of the surgical team share a mental model about the patient and the related risks. A follow-up study demonstrated the connection between relevant clinical markers and shared mental models.

Other aspects of situation awareness have already been introduced within the “core cognitive process model” in an earlier section. Box 6.2 gives examples of reevaluation questions in order to maintain situation awareness.

Decision Making

Decision making is the generic term for the cognitive and emotional processes of determining appropriate information seeking and actions in a changing environment. It involves “ skills for reaching a judgement to select a course of action or make a diagnosis about a situation, in both normal conditions and in time-pressured crisis situations. It … [includes]: identifying options; balancing risks and selecting options [and] re-evaluating” (p. 13). Many aspects of the anesthesia professional’s decision making have already been addressed in the earlier section where the “core cognitive process model” was introduced, so for more information the reader is referred back to that section.

Although decision making in anesthesiology involves many sorts of perioperative decisions, this chapter is particularly interested in the processes used for the non-routine decisions made during the management of problems or crises. This is a very complex process. Although information is sometimes used in what seems to be a purely rational manner, decision making is affected strongly by a number of non-rational elements such as group pressure, ingrained habits, cognitive biases, and perceptual illusions. When determining the alternatives and their various pros and cons the set of options available or their perceived value will also vary.

Traditional concepts of decision making in much of medicine (not anesthesiology) have concentrated on relatively static, well-structured decisions. For example, should patient A with an elevated blood pressure be treated for hypertension with drug X, or should no treatment be started? Other studies have looked at diagnosis as an isolated task (specifically, diagnostic explanation) both in internal medicine and in radiology. These approaches to decision making have not captured the unique aspects of dynamism, time pressure, and uncertainty seen in anesthesiology. Since the 1980s, several paradigms have emerged regarding decision making and action in complex real-world situations.

On the one hand there are models that have their basis largely in research done in the psychology laboratory investigating the limits and pitfalls of decision making under controlled conditions, especially delineating cognitive biases. Daniel Kahneman and his colleague Amos Tversky won a Nobel Prize in Economics in 2002 for this kind of research. While Tversky passed away in 1999 Kahneman continued to work on decision making, and he recently synthesized and summarized the topic in the popular book Thinking Fast and Slow . Two cognitive systems are described. System I is intuitive (heuristic), very fast, less concerned with the precision of the information processing and decision (as long as it is approximately correct) and more concerned with reducing cognitive load. Research about heuristic decision making shows that reducing the amount of information considered can still lead to equally good or superior decisions. System II is analytical, slow but able to consider in detail the sources of information and the fine points of a decision. Especially in time-critical situations humans tend to run on System I as long as possible, sometimes too long—accepting imprecise results when precision is actually needed. Understanding the pitfalls of System I thinking can help professionals to lower the bar for their activation of System II. Another approach to studying and describing decision making, based on observational research on professionals engaged in doing complex decision-making work in real-world settings (or simulations thereof) is known as naturalistic decision making (NDM), with Gary Klein, Judith Orasanu, and others as the pioneers. Models in NDM-oriented work also invoke the idea of parallel systems of decision making, one heuristic and fast, the other systematic, precise, and slow, as well as a number of other aspects of iterative consideration of useful actions.

The core cognitive process model including dynamic decision making (see earlier section on “Core Cognitive Process Model of the Anesthesia Professional”) is based largely on the NDM model, but is consistent with Kahneman’s description of System I and System II. In fact, Klein and Kahnemann have co-written a paper that outlines how their two approaches are in many ways similar, while in a few ways somewhat different. A common (if incomplete) way to describe this is that the work of Kahneman and Tversky emphasizes the pitfalls and errors of decision making, NDM emphasizes how it is often successful despite these risks.

Stiegler and Tung—representing views influenced by the Kahneman-Tversky research—(1) identified current theories of human decision behavior in the context of anesthesia, which are: expected utility, Bayesian probability, formalized pattern matching, heuristics, dual process reasoning, and sensemaking; (2) identified common effects of non-rational cognitive processes on decision making; and (3) suggested strategies to improve anesthesia decision making.

In another publication, Stiegler and co-workers summarized the most common cognitive/non-rational errors detected specific to anesthesiology practice; the leading 10 errors were anchoring, availability bias, premature closure, feedback bias, framing effect, confirmation bias, omission bias, commission bias, overconfidence, and sunk costs. In a simulated emergency setting, the frequency of seven of those errors was over 50%, with premature closure (accepting a diagnosis prematurely, failure to consider reasonable differential of possibilities) and confirmation bias (seeking or acknowledging only information that confirms the desired or suspected diagnosis) being the most common cognitive errors with a frequency of nearly 80% each. In 2016 the Joint Commission also published a paper on the safety issues of cognitive errors and factors that can predispose or increase the likelihood of cognitive biases.

Gigerenzer frames the view on the characteristics of human decision making differently. What is called biases in other frameworks is seen as relevant features of human cognition and decision making that allow for functional perception and action in a very complicated world. Perhaps like NDM, this view suggests that theories of decision making do not really describe decision making in the real world. For example, in many situations it will not be possible to collect all relevant decision alternatives, to assess them fully, and to select the best option. There might not be enough data available about the alternatives and/or the evaluation might take too long. The “take-the-best” (also termed “satisficing”) heuristic suggests that decision makers consider what they see as a key feature for the decision and only compare any alternatives along this criterion. Further criteria are only considered if the first ones do not identify an acceptable option.

Performance-Shaping Factors on the Individual Level

The previous discussion about the performance of skilled anesthesia professionals has mostly assumed that they are normally fit, rested, and acting in a standard working environment. However, human performance on the individual level also depends on so-called performance-shaping factors such as interruptions, distractions, fatigue, and stress. Performance-shaping factors can predispose a person to error. Experience in human performance in the laboratory and other domains suggests that internal and external performance-shaping factors exert profound effects on the ability of even highly trained personnel. The degree to which this occurs and to which it affects patient outcome is highly uncertain. In extreme cases, such as profound fatigue, it is obvious these factors result in severe degradation of the anesthesia professional’s performance or even complete incapacitation. However, these extreme conditions are quite unusual, and it is still unclear whether the levels of performance decrement most frequently seen in typical work situations have any significant effects.

Several performance-shaping factors are potentially of sufficient magnitude to be of concern. Those include ambient noise, music, distraction by personal electronic devices, distraction by other personnel, fatigue and sleep deprivation, aging, illness, drug use, and relatively fixed hazardous attitudes. These are discussed in the course of this section. A variety of other performance-shaping issues such as the level of illumination and environmental temperature are not dealt with in this chapter.

Currently, the responsibility for ensuring fitness for duty rests solely with the individual clinician. In high-reliability organizations (see later), where the maintenance of organizational safety is one of the key elements, the institution implements measures to mitigate decrements due to performance-shaping factors. As health care systems address issues of human performance and patient safety more seriously, these aspects of work will need to be dealt with.

Distractions and Interruptions in the Operating Room

Several recent publications deal with distractions and interruptions in the OR. For detailed information the reader is referred to further literature, as the upcoming section can only give a brief overview on the topic.

Ambient Noise and Music

The workplace of anesthesia professionals—predominantly, but not exclusively, the operating room—is a very complex physical and cognitive setting. Unless there is an unusual effort to reduce sound levels the routine noises of the suction, surgical equipment (electrocautery, pneumatic or electric power tools), and monitoring equipment yield levels considerably higher than in most offices or control rooms. Other sound sources such as conversation and music are controllable. The potential interference of noise with communication and situation awareness among personnel in the OR is particularly worrisome to those concerned with optimizing teamwork in this complex work environment.

The use of music in the OR is now widespread. Many health care professionals believe that music enlivens the workday and can build team cohesiveness when all team members enjoy the music. Others note that in some cases the volume level of music makes it harder to hear the rhythm and tone of the pulse oximeter and alarms as well as work-related conversation between team members. A laboratory study by Stevenson and colleagues determined that visual attentional loads and auditory distractions additively reduced anesthesiology residents’ ability to detect changes in pulse oximeter tone.

A controversial study by two social psychologists, Allen and Blascovich, suggested that surgeon-selected music improved surgeons’ performance on a serial subtraction task and reduced their autonomic reactivity (i.e., “relaxed” them) when compared with control conditions consisting of experimenter-selected music or no music at all. The methodology of this study has been criticized. In response to Allen and Blascovich, several anesthesiologists challenged the notion that the surgeon’s preference for the type or volume of music can or should override the needs of other members of the team.

Murthy and colleagues studied the effect of OR noise (80 to 85 dB) and music on knot-tying ability in a laparoscopic skill simulator. They found no difference in time or knot quality in the conditions tested and concluded that surgeons can effectively block out noise and music. The invited commentary that accompanied the article brings up important issues: What impact does noise have on other members of the surgical team? How does noise affect communication between team members? Does noise affect judgment? The question of the proper role of music in the OR has no simple answer. Clearly, optimal patient care is the primary goal. Some surgical or anesthesia personnel explicitly forbid any type of music in the OR. A more common approach of many OR teams is to allow any team member to veto the choice or volume of music if they believe that it interferes with their work.

Patient Safety Action Box

Distractions include music, social conversation, and jokes. Each of these activities is appropriate under the right circumstances. They can make the work environment more pleasant and promote the development of team spirit, but they can also seriously lessen your ability to detect and correct problems. This issue can be raised during the pre-surgical time-out to ensure that all personnel agree. During patient care you must take charge of modulating these activities so that they do not become distracting. If the music is too loud, one must insist that the volume be reduced or that it be turned off (a rule of thumb is that the pulse oximeter volume should always be louder than the music or conversation). When a crisis occurs, all distractions should be eliminated or reduced as much as possible.

Reading and Use of Mobile Electronic Devices

The observation that some anesthesia professionals have been seen to read journals or books casually during patient care led to a vigorous debate of the appropriateness of such activity. Although it is indisputable that reading could distract attention from patient care, a study by Slagle and Weinger in 2009 suggested that when the practice of reading is confined to low-workload portions of a case, it has no effect on vigilance. Many comments about the issue were related not to the actual decrement in vigilance induced by reading but rather to the impact of the negative perception of the practice and of those who do it by surgeons and by patients, if they were aware of it.

This issue has been greatly magnified by now-ubiquitous smartphones and social media. Nearly all clinical personnel (other than those scrubbed into surgery) have a smartphone immediately at hand. These provide great temptation for both pull activities—reading email, websites, or social media sites—and push activities, notification of new text messages, mail, or social media posts. The wide variety of channels of information makes it far more likely for people to spend time interacting with their phones than with newspapers, journals, or books. The content available is never ending. In their survey, Soto and colleagues give a current update of the usage patterns, risks, and benefits of personal electronic device use in the operating room.

You're Reading a Preview

Become a Clinical Tree membership for Full access and enjoy Unlimited articles

Become membership

If you are a member. Log in here